The Consuming Instinct | Dr. Gad Saad | Talks at Google

Video Statistics and Information

Video
Captions Word Cloud
Reddit Comments
Captions
[MUSIC PLAYING] JORDAN THIBODEAU: Awesome. Hey everyone, thank you all for coming through. Really appreciate it. My name is Jordan Thibodeau. I'm here hosting Gad Saad. This talk was brought to you by Talktopia. Go/talktopia have a lot of great talks on there. Tim Ferriss writing all day and what not. So if you don't want to miss out on a great event, go ahead and subscribe. Gad Saad is someone who doesn't really need an introduction, but I'm going to do it anyways. He's a professor of marketing at Concordia University and a research chair in evolutionary behavioral sciences and Darwinian consumption. He also has an amazing YouTube channel, which is called "The Godfather?" GAD SAAD: "The Saad Truth." JORDAN THIBODEAU: The Saad Truth." People call-- his nickname is the god-- OK yeah, "The Sad Truth." so anyways, thank you all for coming through and let's give a round of applause to Dr. Gad Saad. GAD SAAD: Thank you. Thank you very much, Jordan. It's a true pleasure and honor for me to be here. I guess my life has gone full circle because my doctorate, which I defended 23 years ago, was actually in an issue of "Search," which should be relevant to the top search company in the world-- of course, you do more than that. My doctoral dissertation was on a very specific problem, specifically something called stoppings behavior. When is it that we've acquired enough information to stop acquiring additional information and commit to a choice? Which is a fundamental search problem, right? When you're choosing a mate to marry, of course there are many more people to sample from in the world, yet we don't, regrettably. When it applies to which political candidate we choose, which car we buy, which person to hire, we don't look at all of the available and relevant information. Instead, we look at enough information until a point when we say I'm ready to choose Toyota, I'm ready to marry this person, I'm ready to hire this guy. So this was the topic-- the cognitive processes of how we stop information search. That was my doctoral dissertation at Cornell. Then, I went off. Well actually, let me tell you how it all started the evolutionary part of my research career. When I went to Cornell, it was really to study consumer choice as a mathematical modeler. And then I hooked up with the guy who became my doctoral supervisor, who was a mathematical and cognitive psychologist, and he suggested that I go into the behavioral sciences. First semester at Cornell, I had taken a course with this-- Oh! What did I do? What did I do? OK. I had taken a course with this gentleman-- professor Denis Regan. It was an advanced psychology course. And about halfway through the course he assigned-- this is in fall 1990, my first semester at Cornell-- he assigned this book. Has anybody heard of this book? Has anybody read it? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] GAD SAAD: Sorry? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] GAD SAAD: Looks? AUDIENCE: Cheery. GAD SAAD: Cheery. Yes, exactly. "Homicide" is a book written by two pioneers of evolutionary psychology, Martin Bailey and Margot Wilson, where they offer evolutionary explanations for patterns of criminality to the extent that there are certain types of crimes that happen in roughly the same way across time, across cultures. There might be some underlying universal processes that explain these crimes. And when I saw the explanatory power of evolutionary psychology in explaining criminality, I had my eureka moment. I knew that I would use that framework to study consumer behavior. Now I define consumer behavior very broadly. It's not just consuming Coca-Cola and Starbucks, but we consume religion, we consume friendships, pretty much anything you can think of, I could fit it under a consumatory umbrella. And so this is how I started looking at how I could apply biology and evolution, and psychology in particular, to study our consumatory nature. And so what I'll do for today's lecture is offer you, hopefully by the end of today's lecture, you'll get a good sense of how we could use Darwinian thinking in studying consumption. Now as you might imagine, in the social sciences, I really straddled both the natural sciences and the social sciences given the work that I do. Amongst my natural science colleagues, of course they all say yes, of course biology matters. Amongst my social science colleagues, the story is a bit more complicated, because most social scientists, in a sense, define their belongingness to the social sciences club by their abdication of biology, right? According to them, what makes us humans is that we transcend our biology. And so as you might imagine, I've had quite a bit of a rough ride. Now, the reality is that science is an auto-corrective process. So without being too gleeful, the better ideas eventually went out, and I think most people now are starting to come around. It's becoming less tenable to say openly that you don't think biology matters when it comes to human affairs. But there are still some old remnants who refuse to admit that biology matters. Some of the key canards that you typically hear, some of the key objections-- there are many more of these. I discuss this-- by the way, the book that Jordan kindly got for you guys, "The Consuming Instinct," I discussed many of these detractors, but let me just mention a few very briefly. So here's one typical argument you hear. Well, biology is great to explain the behavior of the mosquito and the zebra and the dog, but surely, you can't use biology to explain human behavior. This is called the human reticence effect. The exact same evolutionary principle might apply to every possible species except humans. Evolution stops right here. Somehow our brains come through a magical process that's outside of evolution, which, of course, is ridiculous. Here's a second one you often hear. Evolutionary psychology is dangerous because it quote "justifies" and condones reprehensible behavior." So for example, if you explain why child abuse might happen or why rape happens or why men and women cheat on one another in an otherwise monogamous union, then somebody will come along in a very irate manner and say you're using science to condone and justify these behaviors. Of course you're not doing that. The oncologist who studies pancreatic cancer is not for cancer. He's not justifying cancer. He's studying honestly cancer. And so if you study child abuse and rape and marital infidelity, that doesn't mean that you're for it. That doesn't mean that you're trying to come up with an umbrella to justify it. It means that you're trying to honestly understand these very reprehensible behaviors. Here's another one that comes up often. This is what I call the but men are not taller than women because Tom-- not Tom Jones. What's his name? What's the actors name? Tom Cruise. Tom Jones. I'm showing my age. Tom Jones was a was a sex symbol in the '70s. So Tom Cruise is shorter than Katie Homes who was his ex-wife, therefore aha! it's not true that men are taller than women. The cognitive bias there is that people take a single datum to falsify a statement that holds true at the population level. So if you explain, on average, men prefer to mate with nubile, fertile women more so than post-menopausal women, someone will put up their hands and say, but that's not true because my Uncle Joe is dating an older woman named Glenda. Oh gee, Darwin is dead. We're back to the drawing boards. Of course that's not true. But again, that's the cognitive vice that people succumb to. And this is the one that irritates me the most because it is the one that typically is levied by otherwise sophisticated academics. They say that evolutionary psychology is just a bunch of fanciful post-hoc stories that you come up with after the fact. Nothing could be further from the truth as I will show you in a few slides. This is going to be a bit the technical part of the presentation. And then later, I'll give you some sort of sexy, practical examples. So the epistemology of evolutionary psychology, or for short, I'll just call it EP. So the method of EP is really comprised of three components. When you're doing evolutionary research, you really are doing these three things. You first have to differentiate-- and I'll explain each of these in turn. You have to differentiate between what are called proximate and ultimate scientific explanations. That's a fundamentally important distinction. And I'll talk about this in a second. You build what are called nomological networks of cumulative evidence, very, very important. Again, I'll explain this. And then you also build what are called consilient trees of knowledge. Consilience basically means unity of knowledge. Physics is more consilient than sociology, not because physicists are smarter than sociologists, but because physics has organized its knowledge base in a way where there are these organized, coherent trees of knowledge. Sociology hasn't. We can't agree in sociology whether there is such a thing as innate sex differences or not. OK? So I'll explain each of these in turn. So let's begin with this first one. So here I'm going to discuss this one. The difference between proximate and ultimate explanations. Much of science operates at the proximate level. Most Nobel Prizes that have ever been won have been won at the proximate level. What does proximate means? Proximate means you're trying to understand the how and what of a phenomenon. How does diabetes work? What are some factors that increase the severity of the symptoms of diabetes? These questions-- the how and what, which is where much of science operates-- those are proximate questions. Ultimate questions-- and by the way here, people think ultimate means superior, it's the ultimate. That's not what it means. Ultimate means the ultimate Darwinian why. Why would the mechanism have evolved to be of that form? So it's not that ultimate explanations are superior to approximate ones, it's that you need both levels of explanations to fully understand the phenomenon. So again, you will only study these types of explanations if you come with an evolutionary lens. Now you might say OK, I'm not really quite following what this is. So let me give you a very concrete example. Take, for example, pregnancy sickness. This is a universal phenomenon that happens throughout time and across cultures. Women, at a very predictable point in their gestation, will experience pregnancy sickness. The reason why we call pregnancy sickness rather than morning sickness is because it doesn't only manifest itself in the morning, although typically, it does. So we call it, more generally, pregnancy sickness. Well, what would be a proximate story to tackle when it comes to pregnancy sickness? Well, how do the hormonal fluctuations of a woman affect the severity of her symptoms? That's approximate question. The ultimate question is why have women evolved this physiological response? Why is it that women, at a very specific point in their gestation, actually experience these very, very ugly and unpleasant symptoms? And the answer from an adaptive evolution perspective, is that this is the mechanism to protect the fetus in-utero from exposure to pathogens. At a very specific point in the gestation called organogenesis, during the first trimester, if the fetus is exposed typically to food pathogens-- things that the woman will ingest, consume-- that could wreak havoc with the development of the organs. Therefore, evolution has yielded a very elegant, adaptive solution. Women tend to be repelled from foods or other sources that have high teratogen values. They'll be attracted to other foods that serve anti-microbial properties. Pickling-- the typical, stereotype of a pregnant woman wanting pickles-- pickling has an anti-microbial property. Women who experience more pregnancy sickness are less likely to have miscarriages. The outcome of the child is greater if the woman experiences pregnancy sickness. So when a woman goes to her OBGYN and he or she will give her a pill to attenuate those symptoms, from an evolutionary perspective, you're doing the exactly incorrect things. Now you might need it because you might have to give a talk that day at Google and you can't run every three seconds to throw up. So from that perspective, from that proximate perspective, it would make sense, but from an evolutionary perspective, it doesn't. So here you see the difference between a proximate perspective and an ultimate one. Sorry, you might not be able to read this very clearly. But this now goes to the second point. Here I'm going to talk about how you build these nomological networks of cumulative evidence. How do you build evolutionary explanations? And is exactly the opposite of this idea that we just sit there, smoking a pipe, coming up with fanciful stories. It's the exact opposite of that. So here we go. So here, I'm looking at the biological roots of toy preferences. Are toy preferences innate-- biological-- or, is it, as social scientists tell us, the means by which little boys and little girls learn about their sexist, arbitrary stereotypes. From the social scientists perspective, biology has nothing to do with toy preferences. The evidence points very much against that position. So what I've done here is I've built a nomological network. What's a nomological network? It's evidence stemming from many, many different lines of evidence that makes it unassailable that there is an inherent biological root to toy preferences. So I took a lot of effort to actually build this nomological network. Let's go through it very quickly. This is from developmental psychology. You could take children who are too young to be socialized. In other words, by definition, they haven't yet reached the cognitive stage where they could be taught by mommy and daddy to play with trucks and dolls, and guess what? They already exhibit those sex-specific preferences. Now you might say ho do you elicit those preferences? It's either usually when they reach to one of the toys or the other, or through gaze studies. So little boys do look at trucks much more than little girls, and little girls will look at dolls and so on. So already, just that one line of evidence is casting doubt on the idea that there is no innate preference involved with toys, but let's go on. Look at this one-- cross- species. Studies done with vervet monkeys, with rhesus monkeys, and with chimps shows the same sex-specificity of toy preferences. That's not looking like it's all due to arbitrary, sexist stereotypes from the patriarchy. Let's go on. Clinical endocrinology. Girls who are afflicted with congenital adrenal hyperplasia-- this is an endocrinological disorder that masculinizes morphological features, that masculinizes behaviors-- little girls who suffer from congenital adrenal hyperplasia, what do you think happens to their toy preferences? They become a lot more similar to boys. It's not looking like it's all due to random socialization, does it? Let's keep going if you're not convinced. Cross-cultural study, Sweden. By the way, are there any Swedes here? No? OK, so then I'll bash on Sweden without-- In Sweden, there's something called the Hofstede score. Hofstede was a cross-cultural psychologist who scored countries along different cultural values. Of all countries in the world, Sweden scores the highest on femininity. So if there is a natural experiment where you could see whether toy preferences in a feminized society would somehow flip, Sweden would be the case. So this person inventoried 40,000 toys in actual bedrooms of little boys and little girls in Sweden, and guess what? They had the exact same toy preferences as everywhere else in the world. Already we've reached enough evidence, but let's keep hammering it so it becomes unassailable. You could do cross-cultural studies with non-western samples, because you might say oh, but Sweden is still a westernized country. So you could go to cultures in Sahara and in North African region that have absolutely no Venn diagram interactions on many of these cultural values, and yet, the sex specificity of toys is exactly the same. Let's keep going. You could look at classics, meaning ancient Greece. And you could look at funerary art in tombstones. What is the art that's depicted? Little boys are depicted with the typical sex-specific toys. And little girls with a typical sex-specific toys. It's not looking like it's due to the patriarchy is it? Pediatric endocrinology. You could take urine samples from little boys and little girls starting at age seven days until six months, and you show that the more testosterone you have, that will affect your toy preferences. I won't go through all of that. I mean, I guess I missed one or two, but you get the idea. By building these nomological networks of cumulative evidence stemming from completely different fields, you then build an evolutionary argument. This doesn't look like it's just random, sort of storytelling, does it? Let's keep going. Here's another example. This one is a bit more controversial because we're talking about mating preferences. Men have an evolved preference for the hourglass figure in women. My goodness, that's very sexist to say. No it's not. Women, by the way, have very clear morphological preferences in the types of men they prefer. For a sexually reproducing species, you would expect that the two sexes would have evolved preferences. It's called sexual selection, one of the fundamental mechanisms of evolution. So let's see whether we can build an adaptive evolutionary argument to explain this reality. Well, it turns out that the hourglass figure medically has been shown to be a reliable cue of fertility and health. So we're not just building these evolutionary arguments while taking acid. There is actually epidemiological studies that demonstrate that if you have an hourglass figure, you're more likely to be fertile. You're more likely to be healthy. And so it doesn't take much of an evolutionist to understand why there'd be selection pressures for men to prefer women who have the body shape of Beyonce more so than a male Olympic swimmer. OK, let's keep going. This, actually, one of these cited studies comes from me. Let me tell you the background to it. A few years ago one of my undergraduate students had finished his course with me, and then came to me at the end of the course and said, hey, I'd like to hang out with you professor Saad and maybe serve as a research assistant on some project. At the time, I didn't have any research funds. I didn't have my chair professorship yet. I said, well, I don't have any immediate money to give you. He said, well, it doesn't matter, I'll do it pro bono. Just give me a project. I just want to hang out and learn from you. So I thought about what project to give him and I contacted him and I said, all right, I think I've got a project for you. How would you like to surf porn sites for a couple of weeks? To which he answered, I love you professor Saad. What did I ask him to do? I wanted to see, speaking of search engines, the Internet allows us to really get access to cross-cultural data very quickly. I said go to as many sites as you can find online where they advertise online escorts-- women advertising their sexual services. My name is Linda, here is my height, here's my age. And usually, they always give you their measurements. Now of course it doesn't matter whether their measurements are true or not. Because sometimes people say, but what if she's lying? Who cares. That's not the point. The point is what is she advertising? Because what she's advertising is what's catering to men's evolved preferences. And so I simply asked him to code the waist and the hip. And, by the way, in case you want to know later if you want to take the measurements, the preferred waist to hip on a woman is 0.7. On a man-- in other words, what women like in a man-- it's 0.9 OK. So 0.7 is that marker. Well, the average across 48 countries that couldn't be any more different than one another was 0.72, exactly within what you would expect. Let's keep going. Here's the hourglass across 286 Egyptian, African, Greco-Roman and Indian sculptures spanning several thousand years. And also looking at Joman figurines-- 155 prehistoric figurines-- guess what? The hourglass figure comes out across all those cultures. That doesn't seem like it's due to the pervasive patriarchy as shown in "Cosmopolitan Magazine." Let's keep going. I won't go through all of these because you get the point. This one is the real killer. You ready for this? Congenitally blind men-- men who've never had the gift of sight-- exhibit the hourglass figure preference. Now you might say, well, how do they elicit those preferences? They do it haptically. They give different mannequins. The congenitally blind men check them out, and then they choose the one that's the hourglass. That doesn't suggest that it's due to media images. Those guys have never seen media images. So again, I won't go through all of these, but systematically, by coming up with data sources from completely different fields, you end up building an unassailable argument. By the way, none other than the great Charles Darwin himself had precisely done that. Now he didn't call it nomological networks of cumulative evidence. This is a term that came in later. For those you're interested, you could check out a paper by Schmidt and Pilcher that have done this. But when he wrote "Origin of Species," the book that introduces the idea of natural selection, that's exactly what he did. He came up with years of observations coming from many, many different fields, which, when put together, makes his natural selection idea seem unassailable. So contrary to what most people who don't understand evolutionary psychology argue, which is oh, we just come up with fanciful stories, it's the exact opposite. The evidentiary threshold that we set before we decide that something is an adaptation is actually much higher than the rest of the sciences. All right, let's keep going. So this is exactly what this curve is showing. Here's the threshold that you'd like to reach in support of your hypothesis. Here's the threshold that you'd like to reach to refute your hypothesis. And basically, each of those squares that I showed you earlier is systematically leading us up to that threshold. So now that was sort of the more technical part of the chat. Now let's kind of give you some breathing room. Are consumers born or made? You often will hear this-- is it nature or nurture? That's actually a false dichotomy. And the example I like to use is the cake metaphor. If you look here, each of these are ingredients that constitute elements of baking a cake. Then, I bake the cake. If I now tell you here, please point to the sugar, please point to the eggs, you can't. It's an inextricable mix of all of it. That's what humans are. For most relevant phenomena, we are inextricable mix of our nature and nurture. That's called the interactioners viewpoint. Our genes interact with the environment. So it's not only the environment as many social scientists think. And no serious evolutionist thinks that the environment doesn't matter, but what we do say is you have to take biology into account. Now of course in my work what I do is I introduce biology to the business school. If anybody here has an MBA, undoubtedly, you can't get you MBA without ever hearing the word biology mentioned. That's crazy because our hormones affect the investment decisions that we make. If we're situationally hungry or not, we decide to make the decision a or b. So all sorts of very basic physiological realities affect the decisions that we make, whether we're employers, employees, consumers, managers. And so what I try to do is systematically introduce evolutionary theory into the business school. I'll skip this part because it might take a bit to cover. Evolutionary psychology is not only about looking at human universals-- things that are the same across cultures. Evolutionary psychology can also be used to explain why cultural differences exist. So for example, some cultures have very spicy foods, other cultures have very non spicy foods. If I were cultural psychologist, I would simply stop at that fact. I would simply point the Malaysians have spicy food, the Swedish folks don't have spicy foods. Good night, everyone. It ends there. An evolutionist says, no, why do culinary traditions evolve to take different trajectories? Is there a fundamental reason that can help us understand why some culinary traditions are spicier than others? In this case, here's a very spicy Mexican soup, and here is a very non spicy, classic Swedish meatball plate. And there turns out to be a very good answer. If you map the spiciness of a cuisine against pathogenic density across different environments, the data couldn't be more perfect. Why? Because in hot climates where there is a greater proliferation of food pathogens, cultures evolve to come up with a solution to a biological problem. Therefore, spices, pickling, smoking, salting are all cultural adaptations to a basic biological problem. So culture doesn't exist outside the boundaries of biology, cultures exist because of biology. Nothing is outside of biology. Here's a cool example to explain something known as the variety effects. The variety effect is the idea that when you go to a buffet, most of us will tend to over eat, some of us more than others because it triggers our penchant for variety seeking when it comes to foods. Now for some animals, they have a very high metabolic rate. So if you are a hummingbird, you have to eat roughly three times your body weight in a typical day just to survive until tomorrow. If you did that as a 200 pound individual, you'd have to eat 600 pounds. Well of course, we don't have that metabolic rate, but yet we still have this penchant to eat many different foods, which results-- for example, if you go to an all-inclusive vacation. Has anybody been to Club Med? Anybody? On average, I think five pounds a week. So if you go for two weeks, I don't care how much exercise you do, while you're there, you're putting on roughly about 10 pounds. Because at an all-inclusive resort, it's almost impossible for you to not succumb to the pull of sampling way more foods than you otherwise would. And so here's very two examples. These are such simple studies, but they explain something so profound. Take M&Ms that are one color or multi-colored. The colorants is tasteless, it's odorless. In other words, it doesn't affect in any way objectively what happens. But which of these do you think people eat more of? This one, because the visual system is being tricked into sampling more because of the variety of colors. Exact same study, but using shapes in this case. Here is one-shaped pasta, multi-shaped pasta. In other words, the pasta is exactly the same. I eat a lot more of this one. Now this is actually one of the studies that perhaps-- I mean, I'm fortunate enough that my work not only receives attention among scientists, but because of the type of work I do, often the media comes calling. This is one of those studies where the media came calling a lot and you'll see in a second why. So I took here a principle from animal behavior, literally called peacocking. In this case, you've got the male peacock, here's the hen, and he starts exhibiting this type of signaling to show the iridescent coloring, the size of the patterns, the size of the tail-- all of this is what's known in biology as a handicap principle. Why? Because for this animal to have evolved such a burdensome tail, this tail actually goes against its survival. It actually reduces the likelihood of him taking flight and avoiding predators. It actually increases the likelihood of a predator seeing him. So how could it evolve? Through natural selection it certainly couldn't evolve. It evolved through sexual selection because it is actually an honest signal of his phenotypic quality. He is saying, look, despite the costs I am carrying for having this tail, here I am. I'm the real deal. Pick me. So I take this idea and then I apply it to human consumer behavior, and therefore, for all of you rich Google types that are buying the Maserati, you're just lowly peacocks. I got you pegged from day one. What are you doing? You're basically saying, look, you little-- pretend they are males-- you can't compete with me, because it requires for you to play at a higher level to reach my signal. By the way, the upper uppers, the highest echelon, actually drive very non-fancy cars. Why? Any ideas? Yes? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] GAD SAAD: No. Good answer. No. It's-- yes? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] GAD SAAD: It's almost that. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] GAD SAAD: Exactly. Especially within their social class. So let me say it. So if I am a billionaire driving a Ferrari amongst my billionaire's circle. It's not really a barrier to entry for their signaling because they could match that as well. Therefore, it is an irrelevant cue to use to demonstrate my honest status. The way that I would do that is I will buy a painting that a three-year-old could have painted and pay $100 million for it. Now that's an honest signal. It has to be wasteful. Just like the peacock still has to be wasteful, throwing away $100 million on something that a three-year-old degenerate could do, now that's impressive. I'm mixing it up for you. You guys are too politically correct. Let's bump it up a bit. OK. All right, so therefore, what I did in this study-- not a three-year-old degenerate, a prospective three-year-old degenerate. Because at three years old, his trajectory hasn't been set yet. So what we did here is we took this idea and we actually-- and I say we, this is with my former graduate student John Vungus-- we actually got a Porsche and a beaten up car. This wasn't imagine you're driving a Porsche, we actually rented a Porsche. And I always tell people, try to get a granting agency to give you money because you write, we're going to rent a Porsche for the weekend for scientific purposes. Well, we did. And what we did for this study is after they drove these two cars, we took salivary assays to measure, what do you think? Testosterone. And what do you think happens? You put a young male in this car and his endocrinological system explodes. Now in retrospect, you all say, of course that makes sense, but guess what? Nobody in the business school is looking at things like that. Somehow hormones magically cease to be relevant when you enter the business world. It doesn't. It's an indelible part of who we are. Let's go on This is not my study. This is from some British psychologist, but along the same lines in terms of the status of a car. You take the exact same guy. You put him in a Ford Fiesta or in a Bentley. You take the same girl, you do the same thing, and you do opposite sex ratings. So for this guy, you ask women to rate just how attractive he is. It's not how attractive as a mate, which is a multi-attribute. Obviously, his physical looks don't magically change if he's in a Bentley or not. Apparently they do if you're a woman. This guy is gorgeous. This guy is a degenerate. But for men, this didn't matter. In other words, men did not evaluate the physical attractiveness of a woman as a function of which status symbol she was associated with. Now, I can guarantee you that I could run this study in 50 countries of your choosing and we will exactly replicate this finding. We will replicate exactly this finding because the underlying phenomenon behind this finding is rooted in evolutionary psychology. It's not as though in Bolivia and in Nigeria and in Japan this finding would be different. Same thing done with a guy being shown next to a shady apartment or a fancy apartment. And a woman-- same thing-- this guy is gorgeous, this guy is not. Men couldn't give a damn where the girl was shown. So you could do this with a million different products and you'll get the exact same phenomenon. This is a study that we're currently-- this is myself with one of my former doctoral students Tripat Gill. We've collected more data since, so this could be probably updated to 2015 maybe. Here's what we did here. We created, ostensibly, an online dating profile. Here's the guy. Here's stuff about me. Here's my favorite possession. The only thing we changed is his favorite possession. He either has a red Kia. No disrespect to any Kia owners, or he's got this Porsche. And then we asked people to rate this guy. Now let me give you the background to how I got the idea to run this study. There was a famous study done in psychology where they brought a guy into a room and introduced him by ascribing different academic statuses to him. Here is Joe Smith. He's a lecturer. Here's Joe Smith. He's an associate professor. And then when he left, they would ask the people to tell their perception of his height. Well it turns out that with academic status, you grow taller. That's why I told my wife, don't worry, you're not married to a short guy. You're married to an NBA star. Because the reality is that your perceptual system gets tricked by this status cue. And so what happened here? This guy was judged as taller-- there was what we called his status elongation effect. He became taller, but only in the eyes of whom? Women. What do you think men rated the guy? Shorter. Which again speaks to the psychology of intra-sexual rivalry. Both men and women denigrate same-sex partners in a way that is perfectly consistent with evolutionary principles. In this case, I am threatened by a guy who may have higher status than me, so I denigrate him. How do I denigrate him? Oh he's driving a Porsche, he must be a little short runt. And so again, you see how all of this psychology is playing into these basic evolutionary principles. Very, very quickly let me do a few more of these car studies. This is a study that was done a long time ago. It might seem dated, but it's actually very powerful, especially in California, especially in a place like the Bay Area here where a lot of people drive fancy cars. So here's what they did in the study. At a red light-- this was a field experiment-- at a red light, they actually put-- these are the experimenters-- either a-- it wasn't these cars, this was then 1968, so I just chose a fancy car versus a non-fancy car. And they simply looked at how long it took a person who was-- wait, this person doesn't know that they're going to be in an experiment. How many seconds when the light turns from red to green do you exhibit cues of impatience? Maybe you flick the light, maybe you honk. Of course people were a lot more impatient when the car that was blocking them was lower status. That again speaks to certain submissive dynamics that happen within social species. In this case, it's happening in a very modern context in the way that we respond on the road. In this case, in another study almost 30 years later, they kept the status of the car constant that's blocking. And then they looked at whether the car that's being blocked would exhibit greater impatience as a function of which car I was driving. If I am in a fancy car, it'll take me less time to be irritated. And again, you could see these types of dominance, submission interactions across many social species. Let's move on. Now so far, a lot of the stuff that I've been talking about it seems as though I'm looking at male-based sexual signaling. But of course, that's not true. Women-- I mean in the human context, and not just human context, but across animals. Women, of course-- or females, in the greater context-- will engage in sexual signaling. So in this case, this is a female chimp. She will exhibit estrus-- that she is in a sexually receptive state-- by sending certain olfactory signals that are very attractive to the males. Her genitalia will become engorged, will become enlarged. And this is basically saying that she is sexually receptive. So I took this idea and I thought, well, how could we apply this principle, which manifests itself across many, many species to the human context and, specifically, to the consumer context. And the idea was to look at how women dress as a function of where they are in their ovulatory cycle. And what do you think we found? When women are in the maximum fertile stage of their menstrual cycle-- when they're ovulating-- they dress more sexily. They beautify more. Now that doesn't mean, by the way, that they look at an ovulation chart and say, oh geez, OK, there's ovulation, let me dress with shorter skirt. It's a subconscious mechanism. I feel more sexy. I feel less bloated. I feel more desirable. My self-esteem is higher. And so all of these mechanisms ensure that a woman is more likely to advertise herself in the mating market when, evolutionarily speaking, it makes more sense to do so. Now these studies are unassailable because they're looking at very basic physiological, hormonal, biological facts. That's why I very carefully selected to do these studies because it makes them a lot more difficult for my social science colleagues to say, oh yeah, but this is just due to learning. This is just due to culture. Because they're so basic in their physiological genesis. Let's keep going. Switching gears. At the end of a successful mating encounter, you might have a child and so this is kind of the segue to this. Here's a study that was done in Mexico in the Yucatan region. When a baby was born, people would say, oh my god, he looks so much like the father, even though objectively that's absolutely untrue. That's absolutely untrue in that the morphological features of a newborn haven't yet sufficiently formed for you to be able to objectively he looks more like the dad or he looks more like the mom. So something's going on. Not only do people say, oh my god, he looks so much like his father, but which side of the family is more likely to say it? The mother's side because the mother's side, from an evolutionary perspective, wants to send a very clear signal to the father that don't worry, this is your child. So before we had DNA paternity tests and before we had the Maury Povich show, where every second episode is exactly that, we had to come up with a cultural mechanism to ensure paternity certainty. And the way we do that is by fakely stating, my god, he looks exactly like you. And by the way, if you're wondering why these two gorgeous, stunning kids are so gorgeous, that's because those are my children. This is Luna when she was very young, and this is Leor. They're now a bit older. They're in Southern California waiting for me to go to the beach. I am from the beach to come to Google and speak. Now I want to tell you a little fun story. I mentioned this story before, but I always love repeating it because it demonstrates the power of evolutionary thinking in understanding our daily lives. These are the actual images, the first images of Luna in utero. So for those of you who are parents, you know that when the mother goes the first time to OBGYN, usually you get those images. Most people are very happy and proud. They celebrate this. They'll put it up on the fridge. We were no different. We put up this image and one day my mother-in-law comes over to our house, looks at-- some people are laughing because they know where I'm going with this. She looks and she goes, oh my god, Gad, the baby looks exactly like you. So I made my daughter famous because this was the first case in science where the maternal side of the family was trying to assure a father of paternity certainty in utero. So I said, should I be worried that you're trying so hard to convince me that your daughter didn't go behind the bushes on me? She looks at me she goes, oh enough with your bullshit science stuff. Look at that. She looks exactly like you. Oh actually, she didn't know it was a she because we didn't know what sex it was-- it looks exactly like you. Here's a study that was actually just accepted for publication. This is Sigal Tifferet. This is myself and two other young folks who helped us out. This is actually the photo of my parents when they got married in 1950. They're still both alive. This was a study where we looked at-- that this is going to speak about paternity certainty. So you might say, how do you apply this in consumer behavior? Watch. The data here is from Israeli weddings where this author along with these guys-- they asked me to come in on the paper to help with the evolutionary story. And this paper by the way is the longest, most circuitous paper that I've ever had in my scientific career before it being accepted for publication. We've worked on this paper I think for more than five years after the peer review process. That's how long it took to get this paper through for reasons related to people's antipathy towards biological-based thinking. So let me explain to you what we did in this paper. We had data from 30 actual Israeli weddings-- Excel sheets-- where every person who attended the wedding, we had how much money they gave. So in other words, in the Israeli context, you don't give toasters and whatever other appliances. You just give money. So Uncle Mordecai, $180, Aunt Hannah, $200. And so we had all this data, and so we tested two things. One of them is pretty obvious for you to guess, the other one is not so obvious. The first thing we looked at is the size of the wedding gift as a function of the genetic relatedness between the giver and the bride and groom. And what do you think happens? To the greater the genetic relatedness, the larger the gift. In other words, people modulate their investment in others as a function of the genetic lineage. Now you might say, well I could have probably predicted this without being a fancy evolutionary scientist. The second hypothesis you couldn't. Here it goes. If you take, for example, your four grandparents, on average, they share-- each of them-- a quarter of their genes with you. In other words, the genetic relatedness is 25%. If you stop the story there, they're all equally invested in you. But there is a second element to the story. It's not just genetic relatedness, it's genetic assuredness. How assured are you of a genetic lineage? Well, your maternal grandmother is assured of her lineage. There is no such thing as maternity uncertainty. Your paternal grandfather has two generations of paternity uncertainty. Therefore, we expect the maternal grandmother to invest the most, the paternal grandfather to invest the least, and the two other grandparents in the middle. So we took this-- the fancy term is differential solicitude-- we took this idea and applied it in the following way. Does the matrilineal side of the bride and groom give larger gifts than the patrilineal side? And the answer is yes. This is the one for matrilineal. This is the one for patrilineal. So holding genetic relatedness constant, when you come from the mother's side, you give greater gifts than when you do from the fathers side. No one would have generated this hypothesis if you weren't coming from an evolutionary lens. You couldn't have even posited that hypothesis. I don't want to spend much time on this. But remember earlier I talked about that conciliar tree of knowledge? This is exactly an example of a consilient tree when it comes to evolutionary theory. You start with some general evolutionary principles that are now unassailable. They've been tested for over 150 years. These lead to what are called middle level theories. Kin selection is the mechanism that explains why people or animals invest in their kin more so than in random strangers. Based on kin selection, I could generate hypotheses about genetic relatedness and genetic assuredness, and then based on that, I just told you the study that I mentioned. Therefore, what evolutionary theory does is it provides you with these elegant, organized trees of knowledge, very much like you would expect in physics and chemistry and biology. And so one of the arguments that I make and some of my evolutionary minded colleagues make is that the social sciences will eventually have to go through the same revolution that biology did. To date, no working biologist says that evolutionary theory is maybe right, maybe wrong. There is no chemistry professor who is for the periodic table and some who are anti-periodic table. The same thing has to happen in the social sciences. For us to have the same impact and same influence, we have to agree that there is a set of core knowledge that after awhile becomes unassailable. And the way you do that is by building these coherent trees of knowledge. All right we're almost done. Let me just read you a few quotes that are very, very powerful to really drive the point home of the importance of biology to everything that we do. This is an exchange that took place on Charlie Rose. If any of you watch it,it's usually late at night on PBS. He brings all sorts of guests and they get into a really fun conversation. This is EO Wilson, who's a very famous Harvard entomologist still alive. One of the biggest scientists of the 20th century. This is James Watson, the co-discoverer of the double helix structure of DNA, Nobel Prize winner. And they're talking to each other. So let me just read it for you. "Psychologist, most of the practicing psychologists and theoretical psychologists and the vast majority of social sciences, who really determine a lot of the intellect that goes into our policies, you know, and our philosophies, our philosophies, have no collection at all to their true nature of humanity or what human nature is at a biological level. This is still a huge gap. And I believe that we will start to fill that in a way that will be useful in years immediately ahead. I see the coming-- the past century was the coming together of chemistry and biology. This century will be the coming together of psychology and biology. " This is exactly what I do in my work. I marry psychology and biology. Now interestingly-- this might interest some of the folks here who are more practitioner-oriented-- practitioners of business are typically a lot less-- they exhibit a lot less animus to these ideas than academics. Because academics are typically vested within their paradigms and therefore, if you come along on your biology horse and if they feel threatened, then they will put up resistance. Well, the practitioner doesn't care. The practitioner simply cares about what works. So if you're an advertising executive and my theories can help you develop better advertising appeals, go for it. And so here's an example from way back in 1964. This could have been written by an evolutionary psychologist. Let me just read it for you. "Because advertising, effective advertising, is an appeal to human fundamental needs, desires, and motivations, it is an appeal to basic human nature. People the world over have the same basic need for food, clothing, and shelter, the same ambitions, the same egotism, and the same temptations. The setting changes, the climate, the culture, the idiom, but the basic human nature is the same. " This is basically evolutionary psychology. It's looking at these human universals. And then this one is the founder of one of the biggest advertising firms of the 20th century. He's basically saying if you want to understand how to communicate, you have to understand human nature. Again, this is very much of a evolutionary argument. Almost done. Before I put up-- let me just-- Sometimes I get people who would say, well, OK, but give me some very clear, practical applications of the fancy scientific work you do. Well here's one, which will be the next slide. If good marketers, whether they know it or not, understand human nature well. If you develop products that are contrary to some basic principle from human nature, the product will fail. So for example, McDonald works well not so much because it gets celebrity endorsers to support it, but it's because it offers you a product that my evolved gustatory preferences expect. Give me fatty, tasty foods. Well it's not difficult to teach most children around the world to like that product because, again, as I said, our taste buds have evolved to like that because of the environment of caloric scarcity and caloric uncertainty that we've evolved in. So here's an example that speaks to this idea. Romance novels are almost exclusively read around the world only by women. There is no culture. And if you can find that culture, send me an email, I'll make you famous. You won't find it because it doesn't exist. There is no culture where men are the predominant consumers of romance novels. Now you might say, OK, so what? Well, if you want to understand female sexuality, study the archetype of the male hero in romance novels. It is the exact same guy. It's a cut and paste job irrespective of which country-- and very different countries. It can be Pakistan. It could be Lebanon. It could be Bolivia. It could be Japan. The male hero is the exact same guy. He's tall. He's a prince and a neurosurgeon. He wrangles alligators on his six pack. He's reckless in his risk-taking, but he could only be tamed by the love of this one good woman. Boom, I just read for you every single romance novel. Not surprisingly, every single cover the guy looks exactly the same. He never looks like this guy. Who? Is this is this some VP of Google? I shall never be invited again here. So this is not because there is a conspiracy to promulgate unrealistic images of masculinity. It's because marketers understand what, in this case, appeals to women's evolved fantasies, and they provide a product that is consistent with that. There was a company-- I don't remember what it was called-- that tried to move away from that. They wanted to create a new line of romance novels that broke the shackles of this. Guess what happened to that company? It died. It died because it had an erroneous view of human nature. It thought that human nature is infinitely malleable when human nature is constrained by biology. So I'm almost done. I think maybe two more slides. This quote right here, I've always joked that if I ever wrote an autobiography of my scientific life, it would consist of one page. Basically, this quote. Why? Because this quote perfectly captures what I have faced in my scientific career. This is JBS Haldane, who is a very famous evolutionary geneticist and also a guy kind of like Mark Twain. He had this beautiful ability with words, great quips. He said, when scientists go through a new radical scientific idea, they go through four stages of acceptance. When they first hear about it, this is worthless nonsense. This is garbage. In my case, you're applying biology to consumer behavior? What kind of heretic are you? You're a moron. Stage 2, as the evidence starts coming in, well, this is interesting, but it's a perverse point of view. Stage 3, this is true, but who cares. Quite unimportant. Stage for, when the paradigmatic wall breaks, oh, I've always said so. I've always loved your work, Dr. Saad. I still have emails-- and I say this with all due modesty-- I still have emails from guys who had written to me when they were at stage one, but they don't know that I've kept that email. Now they're writing to me at stage 4, will you do us the honor? But I thought I was a bullshitter who applied biology. Because that's how science works. It's an auto-corrective process. Hopefully, the good ideas eventually win out. And were almost done. Last slide. Dobzhansky famously stated, "nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution." And if you walk away with nothing other than this one quote, I slightly tweak this to say, nothing in consumer behavior or in business makes sense except in the light of evolution. Here are some of my books. Thank you very much for your patience. [APPLAUSE] JORDAN THIBODEAU: Thank you very much for your time. We're going to go through some questions real quick. If anyone has any questions for the doctor, please head over to the mic right there. If not, I'm going to jump on the dory and we're going to go from there. GAD SAAD: That's either really good news or really bad news. JORDAN THIBODEAU: Oh I got one for you. Can you talk about what male lions and step-parents have in common when it comes to children? GAD SAAD: Oh, very good. Yes. I guess you saw my latest Joe Rogan appearance. Is that where you saw it? JORDAN THIBODEAU: No, from the book. GAD SAAD: From the book, OK. So what Jordan's talking about is the following idea. When you have a pride of lions, usually there is one, two, three dominant males that really rule the pride. And there will be these bands of young males who are now looking to have their own tribe who will consistently test these dominant males. Usually, they will lose until one day nature catches up to the older guys and they're either killed or they're ostracized out. The new males come in, take over the pride. What's the first thing they do? They kill all the cubs. They kill all the cubs because for a species that is going to engage in substantial parental investment-- none more so, by the way, than human males-- across animals and from a male perspective, it makes no sense for me to invest in the genetic package of other males. Therefore, I kill them. And that will lead females coming into estrus. Now I've joked that that's how you get a female lion into the mood. You kill her cubs in the way that we-- in the human case we play Barry White music. Some idiot on my comments wrote, that's not how it works. He actually literally took my words as literally true that that's how you-- of course that's not the mechanism. The mechanism is different. But it is really what happens. You kill her cubs, she comes into estrus. Now how do you link that to the human context? Member the first book that I mentioned at the start of the talk, "Homicide?" The book that sort of led me on my epiphany to apply evolution psychology to consumer behavior. In that book, they talk about how the number one predictor of having child abuse in a home by a factor of 100 times greater than anything else is if there is a step-parent in the house. And this has now become known colloquially in science as the Cinderella effect. Because the story of Cinderella is there is the evil step-mother who loves her biological children, but is differentially nasty to her step-daughter. And so again here, this is an example where people get upset. Because then somebody will raise their hand, well this is bullshit! I was raised by a step-father and he's lovely. But nobody cares about your personal example. It's the aggregate. OK. Smoking causes cancer, but your Uncle Joe smoked and he lived to be 100. It doesn't matter. But again, people have this visceral response against it. But that's an example of how you apply a behavior in lion context to the human context. JORDAN THIBODEAU: Let's have a seat over here. We have a live question. AUDIENCE: With the example of the Porsche. It's an expensive item and it sounds really cool. That's kind of like I guess more of a complex. There's like a cultural, or some sort of higher level recognition there so it evokes like a primitive kind of response. Is there any research into that or anything you've heard about that? GAD SAAD: Not sure I follow the question. Can you rephrase it? AUDIENCE: Yeah, like a Porsche. We know a Porsche is an expensive, kind of sexy object, and having one increases status. But, the recognition that a Porsche is expensive and it sounds really cool and stuff like that, that requires a higher level of processing. So how does our biological-- GAD SAAD: Right. So the underlying mechanism is, of course, not Porsche-related. In other words, it's in the current cultural context that status has become associated with the Porsche that that's what we get. In other cultures, it might be the number of cattle heads that I own. In another culture, it might be whether I'm a famous painter. Picasso was a profoundly unattractive male by any measure. He was short. He was bald. He was overweight. He was not particularly attractive in any way, yet he had an extremely long line up of women who were more than happy to mate with him-- very beautiful women-- because he was Picasso. So the underlying phenomenon is not as though we have genes for Porsches any more than we have genes for pornography. It's that these modern day products tickle evolutionary principles that exist in us. That make sense? JORDAN THIBODEAU: Do the next live question. AUDIENCE: Yeah. So as an individual and consumer, how does one use this knowledge to protect oneself from step-mothers, advertisers and mother in-laws and things like that? GAD SAAD: Well, I mean it's a broad question. I think to use a cliche-- knowledge is power. So to the extent that each of us has a key-- in this case, evolutionary psychology-- to help us understand human behavior, then it's a very powerful thing to have. I will get students who took my course 10 years earlier who will write to me and say, hi professor Saad, I remember the lecture where you talked about the evolutionary roots of romantic jealousy, and I'm currently having such and such fight with my wife because of such and such. And it perfectly matches what you are-- So in other words, it's not as though there is a prescription for every syllable that I utter to a specific practical application, but rather, having that knowledge, allows us to better understand everything. It allows us to understand group dynamics of Google and why I'm going to fight for the corner office because it serves as a status symbol. So in other words, I'm giving you a universal key for understanding human nature. AUDIENCE: Thanks. GAD SAAD: Cheers. AUDIENCE: First of all, thank you so much for giving this talk. It was very interesting. GAD SAAD: Thank you. AUDIENCE: I want to ask you about one of the studies that you talked about-- the wedding gifts. GAD SAAD: Yes. AUDIENCE: My question is, how do you come from-- the data that you've collected-- how did you come to a certain conclusion? For example, in Lebanon, I grew up there. You know if my granddaughter were to get married, my granddaughter is more valuable to the family. So typically, families give more gifts to the female that's getting married than the guy. That might be a reason. There might be other reasons. How do you come to the conclusion that it's an evolutionary thing not something else? GAD SAAD: So several ways to answer that. One is that, as is the case in all science, you have to rule out alternate explanations. If you can't rule out an alternate explanations. Then your data is not as convincing as it otherwise could be. But I also showed you how we build these nomological networks. These nomological networks, not one of the boxes is enough to convince you, but once I show you the eight boxes, it becomes difficult for you to argue against my position. So that's how you do it in evolution theory. You accumulate evidence stemming from many, many different regions, many different lines of evidence, that it makes it impossible for you to generate an alternative explanation to explain the phenomenon. Thank you. AUDIENCE: Thank you so much for coming today. GAD SAAD: Thank you. AUDIENCE: It's been amazing to hear your presentation live and not just on podcast. Second, my question is, through the lens of evolutionary biology, what is your perspective on people that choose to use a plural pronoun such as "they" or "them" since, evolutionary speaking, they're born female or male? GAD SAAD: Right. So I recently gave a-- for those of you who are not familiar with the question that was just posed-- this is really sort of a big issue in Canada right now because there is a bill that was just tabled and passed called Bill C-16 which incorporates gender identity and gender expression as part of the rubric of hate crime. So in other words, if you do or say or not say or not do something, that could be construed as hateful towards say the transgendered, then you could be liable for a hate crime. And to the context of your specific question of gender pronouns, a good friend of mine and a colleague by name of Jordan Peterson got into a lot of hot waters because he said-- and he's a very accomplished scientist. He's a tenured professor at the University of Toronto in psychology-- he said you cannot force me, the government can't force me, cannot compel me, to use specific pronouns like zir and ze and so on. So I appeared in front of the Canadian Senate and I testified regarding that issue. My position is there's a huge panoply of personhoods. Transgendered people exist, the non-gendered exist, the non-binary exists. We should honor everyone. We should treat everybody equal under the law. That doesn't mean, though, that I have to celebrate your personhood by you compelling me to come up with pronouns that are nonsensical. So my personal position is that we're threading on dangerous waters with this bill. My point during the Senate hearing by the way, was every single word that I uttered in today's lecture could be construed as a transgression of Bill C-16. Because when I'm talking, for example, about innate sex differences, someone could raise their hand and say, never in your course did you mention the transgendered or the non-gendered or the non-binary, and that marginalizes me. So literally, evolutionary biology and evolutionary psychology could become transgressive of this bill. So while I respect everyone's personhood, that doesn't mean that it should be anti-science. AUDIENCE: Thank you. JORDAN THIBODEAU: You can have a seat if you want. GAD SAAD: It's hard for me to sit on this thing. I feel as though like it's-- I've got this, you've got nothing. Now it's forever on Google. JORDAN THIBODEAU: It's because I'm sucking it in actually. Your book is critical religion and what are the positive psychological aspects of religion? GAD SAAD: Yes, great question. So in in chapter 8 of my book-- for those of you who took a copy, the chapter is titled "marketing hope by selling lies"-- so that might give you a general summary of what I think of religion. It's very hard to eradicate religion because, ultimately, we are the only being that we are aware of that is aware of its mortality. And to the extent that we suffer from this existential angst, our only solution to that is to subscribe to some religious narrative. Because all religious narratives ultimately offer you a solution, quote, "to this problem." But here I'm torn, because on the one hand, I appreciate the fact that religion has some functional benefits. But on the other hand, if you truly wish to pursue truth, then maybe believing in wrong ideas, even though they feel good, might not be a good thing to do. So I guess my thinking is few people have the testicular fortitude to be nonbelievers because it's a lot easier to be a believer. JORDAN THIBODEAU: What's the most exciting frontier of evolutionary psychology for you? GAD SAAD: Oh great question. I think it is to apply the principles of evolutionary psychology in practical fields. So for example, in my case, I apply it in consumer behavior in business. Now there is the application of evolutionary theory in medicine. Now you might think, oh, but hasn't that been done already? No. Most physicians will never hear the word evolution ever uttered because they work at the proximate level. My good colleague who was, by the way, a guest on my show, Randy Nesse, one of the pioneers of evolution and medicine. And is now trying to make it part of the curriculum of medical school that physicians, future physicians, be trained in evolutionary theory, because there are insights that come from evolutionary principles that you would typically not have access to if you didn't have that lens. There's a field now called evolutionary law where you apply principles from evolutionary theory to understand whether legal codes are consistent with human nature or not. So I think the next frontier is to move beyond the study of mating behavior and so on. It's to take the key of evolutionary psychology and use it to unlock different practical fields. JORDAN THIBODEAU: Going back to you comment on the car-- I have to reflect on my own personal anecdote-- my first car when I was 18 years old was a 1987 Volkswagen Fox. It had about 200,000 miles on it. Went around the sun multiple times I guess or something. The engine was so loud that I could literally have like George Clooney and Angelina Jolie in my car with a pile of cash, and I couldn't get anyone to come inside because the car looked so terrible. But when I would drive to De Anza for community college, I got stopped multiple times for the car I was driving because it looked like a good stop. But then once I upgrade my car to like a Ford Focus and put my Santa Clara University alumni plaque on that, I haven't been stopped for like 10 years almost. So it's really interesting because in your book you talked about if you're driving a high end car, people are less likely to honk when you're late at a red light compared to if you're driving a cheap car, people will honk right away because it's like, who is this person? But I wanted to ask you, you had a point about how birth order affects our psychology in families. Can you talk a little about that? GAD SAAD: Sure. There's this gentleman by the name of Frank Sulloway who is a historian of science and a Darwinists who came up with an incredible idea to explain birth order effect. Typically, when you talk about birth order-- sorry, should I be looking at you or is it OK to look at-- JORDAN THIBODEAU: Everyone. Yeah. I know I'm just too handsome. It's hard. GAD SAAD: Typically, when you study birth order effects, it is studied from the perspective of how parents treat children of different orders differently. So they might treat the firstborn differently than the last born, and that's the genesis of birth order phenomenon. Frank Sulloway flipped that whole script upside down. He argued that no, actually, what drives birth order effects are the children themselves. And let me just explain that very quickly. It's actually an interesting marketing problem. The fancy term is called Darwinian niche partitioning hypothesis. So it basically works as follows. When a child is born, one of the ways that it has to secure maximal parental investment is to convince the parents that it is uniquely different from all the other children. So there are many different niches I could occupy as a child. I could be the good boy. I could be the rebellious boy. So if I'm the first born, all of the niches are unoccupied. Correct? Therefore, I could pick any of them. If I'm the second born, I have n minus niches. As I go down the sip ship, there are fewer and fewer niches from which I could fill. And he argued that that adaptive problem causes later-borns to score higher on openness to experiences, to be greater thinkers outside the box because of this original problem that they have to solve, which is how could I be maximally different from all my other siblings? And whenever I lecture about this, I always end with the following-- can we guess what professor Saad's birth order is? Boom. I was destined for greatness cause I'm last born. I'm the last four. So that's the explanation for the birth order effect. JORDAN THIBODEAU: That caught my eye because my brother is like 6'3", amazing athlete and whatnot. He was getting recruited for college when he was playing for Bellarmine for football, and I was always known as Gabriel's little brother. And I was like crap, well, I'm 5'7" nothing and I weigh nothing, so I guess I'll be the one who either makes people laugh or is either like studying-- GAD SAAD: The guy at Google. JORDAN THIBODEAU: The guy at Google, you're right. So it's funny how that plays out. Can you talk about the mobility in friendship issue the US? How we're constantly relocating and whatnot. And how can we overcome that? GAD SAAD: So that part I was talking about cross-cultural differences in friendships. And one of the arguments that I've often heard-- when I was in graduate school, most of the graduate students who were not American, would always say the same thing-- you could make friends very easily with an American, but the friendships somehow seemed to be shallower. There are sort of more ephemeral. And so I was thinking, well, why is that? I mean it can't be because Americans are more shallow folks. That certainly can't be it. And so the argument that has been proposed is that the greater geographic and socioeconomic mobility in the United States-- today I'm in Atlanta, tomorrow I'm in Mountain View. Today I am a lower economic status, now I'm a dot.com billionaire. That mobility causes people to have a different outlook on friendships, whereas when you come from the old country, meaning Lebanon, Italy, Greece, India, typically the place where you're born, that little village where you were born, is where you're going to typically die. And therefore, that's closer to the evolutionary environment in which we've evolved. The networks are much tighter. They mimic much more the environment in which we evolved, whereas the environments that we see in our modern environment today are not the ones that we have evolved in. That's why in California, for every single of the most banal exchanges, you have a 600 page legal disclaimer form. I remember where we moved from what we moved from Montreal to-- I was a professor at UC Irvine-- the first place that we rented, a townhouse, the rental agreement was probably three times the size of the Camp David Accords between Egypt, Lebanon, and the United States. In 1953, we passed a certain fumigation, you can't sue us if in 17 generations one of your kids develops cancer. You're reading through this, you're saying, I mean, am I renting an apartment or is this like a nuclear reactor? What's going on? Well, the reason for that is because of the anonymity of the current environment that we live in. The social interactions have to be mandated by these contracts. If you're back in Lebanon, well, I just have to shake your hand and that's enough. Now that's not because the Lebanese are more honorable. That's because, if I cheat you once, that's the last time I'm ever going to cheat you because I'm going to be in that environment. So you don't need the 7,000-page contract to mandate relationships. So I argue that that's what drives the cross-cultural differences and the depth of friendships. JORDAN THIBODEAU: For last question, there seems to be a push against dissension in academia. Can you talk about that? Is this a recent development? GAD SAAD: What was the first part? JORDAN THIBODEAU: There seems to be a push against dissension in academia. Is this a recent development, or is this something that's been going on since the beginning of the university system? GAD SAAD: Well it's certainly gotten worse over the past 30, 40 years. And so for those of you who follow my work, I really have two hats. I both have the hat of an academic who publishes in journals and so on. But I also weigh in on many of these issues-- the political correctness, the trigger warnings, the microaggressions and so on-- that are taking place at the university that it is making it very difficult to have interesting debates about controversial topics because everybody is afraid that they might offend somebody. And actually, the topic of my next book-- which I'm currently working on-- is the tentative title is "Death of the West by 1,000 Cuts." I go into what has led us down this quagmire. So no, I don't think it's a recent thing. I think it's taken about 40 years of horrifying faux intellectualism to lead us to the place where we are at. And some of these movements, in case some of you want to know, postmodernism, everything is relative, there are no objective truths. Cultural and moral relativism, identity politics, intersectionality, radical feminism-- each of these movements are perfect anti-science, anti-reason positions. Put them all together, you get a cocktail. You get a melange of utter nonsense that has led us to where we are. So I'd like to think that there's a few of us now that are working to fight against this. The news is still not good because I will often receive emails from professors-- tons of them-- who will write to me we were so inspired by you, thank you for your courage, but shh, they'll never say it publicly. As if supporting freedom of speech, freedom of intellectual diversity is a controversial topic. They support freedom of speech, but they're afraid to state so openly. So we're in a difficult position. But a few of us are fighting very hard to try to reverse the trend. JORDAN THIBODEAU: Well, thank you very much for coming here and speaking. GAD SAAD: Likewise. JORDAN THIBODEAU: You flew across the country to make it here on short notice, so if we can give him round of applause. [APPLAUSE]
Info
Channel: Talks at Google
Views: 112,714
Rating: undefined out of 5
Keywords: talks at google, ted talks, inspirational talks, educational talks, The Consuming Instinct, Dr.Gad Saad, joe rogan dr gad saad, gad saad, evolutionary psychology, joe rogan experience
Id: _qHYmx7qPes
Channel Id: undefined
Length: 82min 32sec (4952 seconds)
Published: Tue Oct 03 2017
Related Videos
Note
Please note that this website is currently a work in progress! Lots of interesting data and statistics to come.