17. Human Sexual Behavior III & Aggression I

Video Statistics and Information

Video
Captions Word Cloud
Reddit Comments
Captions
Stanford University. --started. See some exam-related announcements. First off, there are the statistics, mean and median, which, in terms of numbers and what they mean-- what they mean in my book is that people are doing a good job in here. So, good going. Also having done an amazing job are the TAs, who, other than coming to class here, have not been allowed out of the conference room since you left your exams with them. They have screamed with the repetitiveness of grading the same question 550 times. And they worked really hard to get this back to you guys in time for decisions about pass, no credit-- that sort of thing. So these guys did yet another amazing job. [APPLAUSE AND CHEERS] Yay, TAs. Let's see. The other thing is, the exams will be outside, at the end of class. A to M is upstairs, and the rest of those letters are downstairs. So start figuring out which you are. The TAs will be flinging boxes of exams around out there, so go get yours afterward. Again, people did a good job. OK, picking up on our by now sickeningly familiar time core strategy thing, here, we left off on Friday, way to the left, in our very last area of sexual behavior and the logic there is-- ooh, somewhere along the way we mentioned genes, or we mentioned a hormone, or a receptor, or whatever, and thus were immediately talking about the evolution thereof, all the way back. What does natural selection theory tell us about the evolution of sexual behavior? And what we've been doing is bouncing between the two genders, in terms of strategies, starting off with the basic asymmetry of caloric expenditure on sperm versus an egg plus a pregnancy, plus, in most species, raising the offspring. All of that giving rise to the famed promiscuity on the part of males-- the lower levels of pickiness. What we saw was the role of male-male competition and making sense of behaviors that are involved in passing on more copies of genes. We have also seen all sorts of male strategies for decreasing the reproductive success of competitors and, in the same process, decreasing that of females for the future. And female counterstrategies in a lot of those cases. What we also then transitioned to was what female choice was about, even in species that were tournament species. And what we transition to now is looking at aspects of female-female competition, in terms of sexual reproduction. What you see is-- predominantly in species that are pair-bonding, that are monogamous-- what you see is the potential for a great deal of female-female competition for access to males. That ironic turn of events where, if the whole system works-- where you give birth and the guy who believes he's the father is going to be taken care of the kids, and what you want is someone who is good at that, is confident at that, can act maternal in a convincing way, and thus is subject to a whole lot of competition on the part of females. And thus what you get is selection pressures in those species-- New World monkeys, for example-- a lot of bird species-- selection pressures. Where, if anything, it's the females who wind up being more aggressive than the males. The females who tend to have the larger body size, who have the more pronounced secondary sexual characteristics. What's that about? It's females competing with females for males to pick them, exactly the opposite scenario as in most of the species we've been hearing about. OK. What this then brings us to is the next great question, which already I've had a lot of people ask me during breaks-- all the way back way there, in terms of making sense of the evolution of this type of sexual behavior and that type and the other type, in terms of passing on copies of your genes and you and your relatives-- all of that. Which brings you, inevitably, to the question of, well, what about the evolution of homosexuality? Because this has always been the puzzle in the book of zoologist, evolutionary biologist, et cetera, trying to make sense of all of this in the context of adaptation and passing on copies of genes, especially when you see how widespread this is in lots and lots and lots of other species. What's up with that? Because, up until relatively recent times, across virtually every culture out there, gay men had a much fewer number of copies of genes, on the average, that they were passing on. How could this trait to be so universal? And best estimates are 5% to 20% rate in every human culture ever looked at. So what's the selection for that? Or why hasn't that been selected against? Basically, there's been three theories that have floated around. And all of them are predicated on one very simple fact that still is not all that factual, which is that there is a genetic component to one's sexual orientation. And we already heard all sorts of bits of evidence about prenatal endocrine environment that argues in other directions. We have already heard that there have been those studies which find covariance of sexual orientation in identical twins versus nonidentical-- finding a genetic marker, but one that nobody else is able to replicate. So everything now is predicated on the notion that, well, genes have at least a little bit of something to do with it. So, three theories that have floated around, first one being the heterozygotic-vigor argument. Lots of folks will recognize that from other realms of genetics. That's the deal where you can have certain traits which can be in the homozygotic form or in the heterozygotic form. And people who are new to this, go back and check your notes from the Mendel genetics catch-up sections. And what you see is, with lots of diseases you get an extreme version is indeed a disease and a partial version is, in fact, adaptive. And the classic example that always gets trotted out is sickle-cell anemia-- full-blown homozygotic version, horrible hematological disorder; partial, heterozygotic version, resistance to malaria and a gazillion diseases out there. And what's done is metaphorically running the same argument of saying, whatever gene genes are relevant, maybe the homozygotic form is the one that produces a behavioral phenotype which decreases reproductive success. But there's some heterozygotic form which has some huge advantage and enough of one to outweigh the one-fourth of the relatives who wind up with the homozygotic version. So that's one model, for which there is very little evidence. Next one. Next one is a gender-dependent genetic argument, as follows. It is a trait. It is a genetically influenced trait which, when expressed in one gender, is maladaptive and decreases reproductive success. But when it is expressed in the other gender, it is highly adaptive and increases reproductive success. And you can immediately run your numbers argument there that, as long as the benefits for the other kind of sibling or gender is larger than the detriment here, this is going to be selected for. What would that look like? Where is the evidence for that? What one would predict is that you would see, for gay men, that their sisters have a higher-than-average reproductive rate. And that is absolutely there in the literature. So the people who push for this view would say, aha, there is some sort of trait which, in a male, increases the likelihood, manifesting as homosexuality in a female, as some route by which there is increased reproductive success. The third model is the "helper at the nest" model, which is that the individual who traditionally is not passing on copies of their own genes directly-- instead, what they're doing is expending resources on helping their siblings. OK, so that kind of sounds like the second model, also. What's the difference? The second model says that sisters of gay men should have increased reproductive rates. What the third model-- the "helping at the nest" model-- suggests is, both sisters and brothers should have increased reproductive rates. And that's actually what's mostly been seen. So, support for this "helper at the nest" kin-selection kind of argument. OK. What else does one look for in a mate, besides all of the stuff that males are competing about and females in pair-bonding species? What are some of the characteristics that pop up over and over again, when it's getting around to who looks good to you, if you are a damselfly trying to pick out a mate, and in all these other species? One initial characteristic, which is the famed role of facial symmetry in attractiveness. This has an interesting history, and this goes back to the 19th century. And there was some guy-- who was it? It was Francis Galton. Was it? Maybe. And it was Francis Galton, I think, who was having-- no, it couldn't be. They didn't have-- forget that. Don't write that down. There was some guy in the 19th century who was a famous criminologist and was pushing a classic nonsense theory of the time, which is that there is a criminal face and that people who are really skilled police people could to look at somebody's face beforehand and immediately predict that this is someone who is going to commit a criminal act, inevitably, at some-- all sorts of nonsense. Genetic determinism and racism and [INAUDIBLE] biases of all sorts running through there. Complete nonsense-- yet one of the dominant intellectual models in criminology in the 19th century was that there are facial characteristics that are typical of criminals. So this was this individual who decided to come up with the archetypal face of what a criminal looks like and managed to get photographs of the faces of a whole bunch of different criminals, pulling them out of the jail or whatever and taking a picture with the newly invented camera. And, through some technique that clearly was some proto Adobe Photoshop sort of ancestor thing, was able to overlap the faces in order to come up with a composite face. And this composite face was now going to tell you, this is the most criminal-looking face that anybody has ever seen. And that's not what happened. Instead, everybody looked at that face and said, hey, that guy's kind of good-looking! That guy is, in fact, really good-looking. And had just stumbled on this interesting thing that, when you average faces, when you toss them all together, that when you merge them, they look more attractive than the individuals that they came from-- when you make composite pictures. And this has kept all sorts of people happy doing experiments where they do mixes and matches of starting with 100 different pictures of people and combine subsets of them and having people rating attractiveness and stuff. And it's this absolutely bizarre thing-- that the more faces that go into a composite, the more, on the average, people perceive them as being attractive, amid incredibly subtle differences between a composite picture of 50 faces versus 25, all of that, people are able to pick up the differences-- not necessarily consciously. So what is that about? And what became clear, many decades later, is that what happens when you make a lot of faces all into one composite is you get a highly symmetrical face. And this seems to be the key thing that comes through in that realm of attractiveness. Which is that, when you average in a whole lot of faces, one of the things you average is the averageness of the face. And you get rid of asymmetries. And everyone knows that-- who would you want to mate with? [LAUGHTER] OK, so, thereby proving the point. And what you've got is, symmetry is apparently a good, reliable marker of health. Wildly asymmetric faces are typically the result of complications-- developmental ones-- health sort of issues. So the general interpretation is that symmetry is attractive because it is a marker of health. And every single article you will ever read in sort of the lay press-- or even in some of the science journals, which should be above this sort of thing-- will have the picture of some famously attractive somebody or other, with the face up there, and all the little caliper markings, and showing the same sizes, and then some poor schnook who's chosen to be the asymmetric face who's stuck up there. Did the Newsweek article wind up being in the readings? No, it wasn't-- yes. OK. Lyle Lovett-- is that the face they had there that they were making vicious fun of and pointing out lip asymmetries and such? Something about the symmetry seems to be a marker for health and is considered more attractive. And people can pick up astonishingly subtle asymmetries in faces. Babies, at two months of age, show-- when you are doing morphed faces, composite faces, where you can very minorly adjust the degree of symmetry or asymmetry in the face-- babies, at two months of age, are already preferring to look at pictures of more symmetrical faces. You find this in lots of other species, as well. When you look at animals lever-pressing-- rats, for example, getting access to other rats of the opposite gender. And you will see there's a bias for symmetry there. There is a bias in nonhuman primates for more symmetrical faces. How's this for a nutty study that was published about five years ago, in Nature? And this one reported that people who had symmetrical faces were better dancers. OK, let's work through that one. What was that one about? And they did-- it was a bizarre study, but somehow, nonetheless, it was irresistible. Here's what they showed. They filmed all sorts of people dancing. And, no doubt, they did perfect controls and they, like, made sure everybody danced to the same thing, which was, like-- oh, I don't know-- something-- a scientist rapping to something or other. And people dancing away, there. And then they used this amazing technique, which I didn't understand in the slightest, with some sort of camera-capture technique that would make everybody dancing look like Gumby. So you took out all the individual features-- anything at all. And people would then rate them on the dancing. And it would turn out that people who were rated by both genders, of either gender, as being a better dancer, when you then went and checked out their faces afterward, with your slide rule, turned out to have more symmetrical faces. Whoa! Isn't that bizarre? My guess would be that this is one of those indirect routes that we've already seen some examples of. My guess would be, more symmetrical faces are thus more attractive, and thus such people are treated better and are more confident in life and more extroverted and more comfortable dancing when they knew that they were going to wind up looking like Gumby. So that is probably what the indirect route is. But running through all of this is this symmetry business. And it's there in species after species. And humans are amazing at picking up subtleties at it. One additional finding in the symmetry world-- women at the time that they are ovulating have their faces become ever so slightly more symmetrical. [COMMENTS AMONG STUDENTS] OK, well, that's going to get people all nutsy in the coming weeks, no doubt, trying to figure out what that's about. I did not see a good explanation as to what that was about, but that has been observed in the literature. So the first thing, in terms of what one looks for in a mate, if you're some basic social animal-- first set of features, this business about symmetry. Next realm-- this whole world of secondary sexual characteristics. Why is it that female peacocks like male peacocks to look like male peacocks, and all that sort of thing? What is that about? And people have been working with this one for years. And there is a zoologist named Zahavi who has been very influential-- Israeli zoologist-- who came up with what is called the "handicap principle." As follows. Why is it that secondary sexual characteristics are appealing? Characteristics that exaggerate the difference in appearance by gender. Why is that appealing? Because, in effect, the bigger and more garish and the more over-the-top your secondary sexual characteristic is, the more you were communicating to the world, I have so much energy on board, and I am so healthy, that I can afford to squander massive numbers of calories on these asinine neon antlers I'm walking around with. That it's an argument there that this is a display of, in a sense, conspicuous consumption-- the ability to put energy into these large secondary sexual characteristics. That these are markers of health and of good immune systems. Lots of evidence for this. And this is, by now, a whole field that people have studied. One version of it. These were studies done with marsupial mice. And there was something having to do with their marsupialesque reproduction that made this the species to do this with. But it was doing sperm insemination of females. And what was done was that they took the sperm from these male marsupial mice, after measuring some secondary sexual characteristics in them. I believe that they don't have antlers, but I don't remember what the actual thing was. And what they were able to do was to now have sperm from males of differing degrees of exaggerated secondary characteristics. And what they showed was that the males who had the more dramatic secondary characteristics-- their sperm were more fertile. So that was a first big vote in this handicap principle that things like secondary appearances, secondary sexual ones, can be a marker for health and/or fertility. Now why should that be? What you essentially are seeing there is, it is a display. It is a potlatch. It is a display of how much energy you can afford to waste and thus how much you must have on board. It is a marker of health and good immunity. This has been shown in lots of studies. Along the lines of, the same sort of signals that give rise to the secondary sexual characteristic are markers of immune function. A well-functioning immune system will be generating some molecule which exaggerates facial coloration. And that's the secondary sexual characteristic-- that there is an explicit link between immune function, immune competence, and these markers. Interesting. A study that just came out recently, looking at women from, I believe it was, about 20 different industrialized countries. And what was shown was that, in general, women have a preference for faces of men that have strong pronounced secondary characteristics-- which is, big, jutting jaw, high forehead, lots of muscle stuff going on. And what they showed was that the longer the life expectancy in the particular country, the better the economy, the better the quality of life, the less women in that country had a bias towards male-looking males. Which is extremely interesting. What's the comparison there? It was all those, like, Scandinavian countries. Thank god for the Scandinavian countries, because they always provide the extreme of these distributions. So, in those Scandinavian countries, like, there has not been a guy who's been, like, selected for a date because of the height of his forehead, in centuries. Whereas what they showed was, in a lot of other cultures-- which were the ones who were most extreme on the list, I am not remembering, which is not very helpful. But a very different sort of end of things. And there, much stronger selection for what are viewed as traits, as markers of fecundity and health. More evidence about this. What you see is, all of this is a signal-- all of this is a signal, not only in terms of who you want to mate with, in terms of who you are passing on your copies of genes in cooperation with, but also the possibility that the other individual is infectious with something or other. And throughout the world of social beasts, nobody likes getting sexually transmitted diseases. And a lot of what this whole handicap principle is about, a lot of this advertising about what a great immune system you have, is a way of advertising, I do not have a communicable disease. All sorts of species-- individuals are extremely good at detecting the smells in the others of parasitic infections, of all sorts of other infections, and avoiding them like the plague. A common theme in lots and lots of species. Rodents, where it's been most studied-- a remarkable ability to smell out the health of another individual. And this is someone you want to stay away from-- that they're going to give you an STD. So that's part of it, as well. So, of course, what you start getting are individuals trying to cheat at all of this. And this is suddenly the world of selection to uncouple your secondary sexual characteristics from your health. Is there a way to cheat? Is there a way? And what is seen in some bird species is, you've got some guy on his deathbed, there. And what's he doing with his last 3 and 1/2 calories worth of energy? He's expending it on wing coloration or whatever the secondary characteristic thing is, there, that he's got four minutes of life left, and maybe there's one more chance to pass on copies of his genes. You get cheating, as well. You get cases of disproportionate shunting of caloric investment towards some of these markers amid animals being sick. So, of course, you have to have counterstrategies. And we have yet another world of coevolution between some sort of exploitative strategy and a counter one to try to detect that. Then you've got one problem with this whole literature, which is, there is a certain way to frame things so that you never, ever can get a finding that disagrees with your general stance. OK, here's how it goes. Here's the version that we've just heard of this handicap principle, which is, what you have to have is your secondary sexual characteristic-- the intensity of it has to be reflective of the quality of your immune system. And in lots of species, facial coloration is generated molecules from the immune system in a linear relationship, there. Yes, that is a way of advertising, I've got a great immune system. But get this. Meanwhile, you can have exactly the opposite case, which is, you can have your secondary sexual characteristic driven by an infection that you have. And what you are presumably communicating is, if I'm still able to do this ridiculous courtship dance, despite the fact that I'm teeming with parasites, I've got the greatest DNA going around, there. Hey, how about it? Where you see this is in vultures. And the mere notion that vultures having sex, and let alone them having sex based on thinking some of them are more attractive than others, leaves me breathless with admiration. But what you see is, in vultures-- I'm forgetting which species-- sexually dimorphic-- the males have more colorful faces. They tend to have these orange faces made out of carotenoids-- these pigment things. These carotenoids. So one version that we've heard already is, OK, where are the carotenoids coming from? They're produced in the immune system. And the happier your immune system, the more carotenoids you pump out. That would be the first scenario we've heard. In vultures, you see exactly the opposite. Which is, where-- this is going to go downhill really fast, now. So where do you get carotenoids, if you are a vulture in the plains of East Africa? You get them by eating ungulate feces. Which vultures are perfectly happy to do, if it's fresh-- or even if it's not so fresh. And what that will typically contain are lots of parasites. Consuming a whole lot of ungulate feces, rather than, say, ungulate muscle, increases the likelihood that you have some sort of infection that you've picked up. What's the interpretation there? Yet here I am, bouncing around happily and energetically. Just imagine how great my genes are, if I'm able to do that amid just teeming with parasites, because of my unlikely sort of dietary habits. So you've got it, here, that it can go both ways. Ooh, dramatic secondary sexual characteristic is a marker of a good immune system-- I can fight off disease. Dramatic secondary sexual characteristic as a marker of, look how well I'm functioning, here, despite teeming with some disease. You get it both ways. It remains very, very controversial-- for the three and a half people who deeply care about this. One additional version of that-- and the "there's no free lunch, and there is no free ungulate"-- whatever-- OK, one additional version of that are studies now showing that in lions what you get is-- highly tournament species, and thus a highly dramatic set of secondary sexual characteristics. The manes. The manes on the lion. And what everybody knows from The Lion King is that they can be very big and dramatic-looking and thus make you very attractive. Unless you're hanging out with hyenas. But what is clear, also, is, it comes with a price. What is the most attractive sort of male lion you can see? It's a black-maned male. Because that is one that is most expensive, in this handicap-principle sense. But there's a downside, if you've got a black mane, which is, your head heats up more, sitting there in the African sun. You've got to spend more energy on thermal regulation. There's a downside to it. All of these things, where you're balancing. What else? What else, in terms of attractiveness? So we've now got, lots of species, the symmetry business as a marker for health. The secondary sexual characteristic nonsense stuff as a marker of health but interpretable in a number of different ways. Then there are the secondary sexual characteristics that are more directly markers of fertility. And what we've entered into, here, is the world of primate species, where females have external swellings when ovulating, when there are visible signs of estrus, of going into heat. Humans are concealed ovulators. And we already know some of the theories about that. But all the other primates get explicit, observable estrus swellings when it is time that they're ovulating. And all you need to do is look at baboons. And beginning somewhere around junior high, when they start hitting puberty, some females have bigger swellings than other females. And it turns out, these stupid, shallow male baboons prefer the ones with the big swellings to the ones with the small swellings. You can even show, with captive male baboons, that they will lever-press more to get to see pictures of female baboons with big swellings instead of middle-sized ones or little ones. There you go. Somewhere out there, there is a magnificent reality show to be set up along those lines. What you see with that is, what's the swellings about? It is a marker of estrogen levels. It is a marker of-- the higher the estrogen levels, the more the swelling. And what you wind up seeing, in various studies by now, replicated, showing that, among female nonhuman primates, those females with larger swellings have a better infant survival during the first year of life of the kids. It's a marker of greater fertility and greater health. And there's maybe some element of a handicap principle there, because by the time you get female monkeys with the biggest swellings around, they now weigh about 25% more than they weigh the rest of the time, all because of the water retention. And presumably part of the signal there is, if I can be running around in my female-in-estrus sort of way, despite 25% more weight sloshing around at the end of me, just imagine how strong and healthy I am. So that, as a source of attraction. Meanwhile, over at the male end of things, in terms of looking for other markers of attraction in females-- by the time you get to humans, we are not, again, external ovulators. But what you have with humans is the famed waist-hip ratio measure. And this one has been endlessly studied and argued about. The notion that the larger the size of the hips, relative to the size of the waist, the greater the waist-hip ratio-- no, the lesser the-- flip that around the other way. OK. The bigger the hips are, relative to the waist-- this is a marker of fertility. That is a marker of childbearing pelvises. That is a marker of all sorts of developmental health that augurs well for having a baby pass through the birth canal. In culture after culture, men find women to be more attractive who have a higher-skewed ratio of hip to waist. So that's been studied all over the place. What you have, though, as the central controversy in that field is, if every single culture that that's been studied in has been contaminated by their exposure to westernized culture and its pervasive values, which may be the source of that. So, among the hip-waist sort of zealots, what has been the Golden Fleece to go after, what has been the equivalent of getting identical twins separated at birth, what is the thing they go after, is to find a population of humans who are having their first contact with the outside world and have had no exposure to the westernized world. And thus you are able to rush in there, quickly master their language, and, once you've done that, you get to ask them the first question in their entire content with the westernized world, which is, so, which of these look better to you? Asking the guys, there. And, at this point-- not surprisingly, there is not a huge literature of asking people, on the first encounters, about hip-waist ratios-- what you see is, the literature is mixed. When you look at more traditional societies, you do not necessarily see as strong of a hip-waist-ratio set of opinions. But nonetheless, at least some of that is universal in every culture that's been looked at. Again, as a marker of fertility. Meanwhile, over at the female end of things, you've already heard what some of the things are that are looked for, in terms of responsiveness to secondary characteristics in males, which is jutting jaws, big, high foreheads, and muscle mass. And all of these are assays indirectly of testosterone levels or testosterone exposure or sensitivity during adolescence. Those are preferred. Interesting thing. Studies showing that, when you give women a choice of faces of males that have been manipulated, changing the dramatic-- how dramatic the secondary characteristics are-- like forehead and jutting-jawedness and angularity of face and stuff-- what you see is an interesting dichotomy. Which is, in these studies, women on the average rate the rounder-faced pictures of men, such men, looking more likable, more honest, more trustworthy, and less desirable. OK. So this is hopeless. And what do you know? Those-- well, I won't go there, either. What those show is separable traits, there. Separable traits, in terms of what's attractive-- what information. And, again, when you look at these studies, the differences are, like, 5% difference in nostril width and things, when they're manip-- these are extraordinarily subtle differences, where people are not consciously aware of them. Which face looks more attractive? Which face would you like more? Which face would you trust more if they told you to vote for somebody or other? An interesting separation, there. OK. One additional one is that, in women, when women are ovulating, they prefer more sexually secondary, secondary-sexually characteristically dramatic male faces. At the time of ovulation, women rate as more attractive male faces with the juttier jaws and the greater muscle mass and the forehead stuff going on. What we see here is this theme of, at the time that estrogen levels are at their highest in humans and lots of realms, in addition to other species, detection of pheromones from males is the most sensitive. Ability to pick up subtle differences in facial symmetry or secondary sexual characteristics are at their best. And the preferences are derived from that. This is, in some cases, some rather substantial effects. OK. So, what other things? There's one problem that runs through this entire literature. OK. So here's the deal. You are a-- uh-- you're a gazelle. You're a female gazelle, and you have read lots of zoology and you know all about this handicap principle. And you know the fact that big, dramatic secondary sexual characteristics on guys is a marker of better immune systems and more fertile sperm and all sorts of great stuff. So you go out of your way to go and find one of those guys. You mate with him. And you've just given birth. So, knowing what you know, what you, of course, now realize is, you've got a great kid on your hands that have all these terrific genes from that jutting-jawed antelope guy you mated with. And you better make sure this kid survives, because they have an enormous potential for having a big reproductive success later on. What do you do? You expend more calories in taking care of them. This has been shown in all sorts of species. Mate various birds in various bird species-- mate females with males who are more attractive versus mating them with less attractive, and what you see is that the females who have mated with the more attractive males give birth to bigger eggs. That's interesting. OK, so you're seeing the evidence of the good genes coming from the guy. Male genes have nothing to do with egg size. It is how much protein the mother puts into the egg production and the egg development. And what you see is, she makes this a self-fulfilling prophecy. In a lot of species, now, it has been shown that when females mate with more attractive males they put more effort into having the offspring survive. The offspring survive better. And yes, what do you know? You really do want to mate with guys with big antlers and that sort of thing. This big problem with self-fulfilling stuff. So that's a confound that has run through the field. Another confound-- and here is one that shows how many species are far less distant from us than one might think. This was work by a guy named Lee Dugatkin at the University of Kentucky. And what he does is study ospreys or some kind of bird thing that runs around. And what he does is, he first gets a male and female osprey, and he introduces them to each other. And what he looks for are circumstances where the magic does not occur. There is no chemistry between them. The female rejects the guy. And, at that point, what he does is, in the next round, he makes the male appear to be very popular. He puts the male here, and he takes a whole bunch of stuffed female birds and puts them around in a circle around him, gawking at him with rapt admiration and just frozen in their place at the wonders of, like, the coloration on his bill from eating who-knows-whose feces. And what you see then is, the female who spurned him is now more likely to do solicitive courting gestures with him. In other words, she's jumping on a bandwagon. And this has now been shown in a number of species. What's the logic of this? This is exactly what secondary sexual characteristics are about. I don't understand why anyone considers that to be attractive. This individual does nothing for me. But if it appears to be the case that those traits are very popular, and thus those traits are predictors of being able to pass on many copies of your genes, I sure want my kids to have those traits. And thus this bandwagon effect. So all of these are versions of just shallow, shallow, horrible-values versions of who one looks for in a mate. But then, of course, we go to all the pair-bonding species. And as we heard from many lectures ago, the rituals in lots of those species of male courtship built around showing that they are a competent parent. Showing that they can do some rough approximation of acting like a mother to these babies. What's that about? And that's the whole world of male birds doing courtship by bringing worms and feeding the mother and showing, look, I know what kind of things we eat. I know how to bring a worm. By the time you get to pair-bonding species, the rituals and what is attractive are often markers not of fertility but of parenting skill-- parenting competence. Flipped the other way, in most nonhuman primate species that have been looked at, when are females most attractive to males, independent of the size of their estrus swelling? When they've already had a couple of kids. Females, the first-time-out rounds of ovulating, are less preferred than females who have had a number of kids already. If it is the same thing going on, what she has already proven is she's competent enough that she didn't kill the kids by dropping them out of the tree at various points. This being as a marker, again, of competence. OK. Final realm of what one looks for in a mate, across lots and lots of different species. Which is not only someone who appears healthy and symmetrical, and these exciting secondary characteristics that are markers of lots of fertility, and also whatever everybody else thinks is attractive, you suddenly do as well-- the last thing that tends to be a theme through lots and lots of species, and in every human culture, is being attracted to someone who's kind of just like you are. Being attracted to someone who is similar. And the term for this is "homogamy." Homogamy, polygamy, monogamy. In this case, homogamy-- mating with someone who is homogeneous, who is similar in traits of yours. And across cultures, including in the United States, people are extremely homogamous in who they wind up marrying. Here are the statistics. When you look at couples in this country, there is better than a 90%-chance likelihood of them sharing the same religion, of being within three years of age of each other, of sharing the same race-- the ethnicity-- sharing the socioeconomic status of their childhood-- did they both grow up poor, wealthy, whatever-- and sharing political views. More than 90% concordance in this country on those traits-- markers of homogamous preference for someone who is just like you. Stepping down a bit-- not in this range-- but more than 40% homogamy for a couple having IQs that are within, I don't know, five points of each other-- having similar levels of education. Those are very homogamous traits. Then a whole bunch of weirdo minor ones which, nonetheless, are still statistically significant. A 20% to 40% likelihood of couples being the same height-- not the same height, but say, for example, in the top 10 percentile of height for their sex-- scaled to what's within gender. Weight, hair color. And then you get into this bizarre world of lung capacity, of width of nostrils, of width of eyes. People have studied these, and these are traits that show significant homogamy. In this fairly low range, but nonetheless significant at a higher-than-expected rate. What are these weird things being about? These are probably surrogate markers for preferring people of similar race, similar ethnicity. That's probably where those numbers are coming from. So what's this about? This is suddenly barreling us back into the world from many lectures ago of, you don't want to mate with someone who shares half their genes with you-- the dangers of inbreeding. But you also don't necessarily want to mate with someone who is so unrelated that there is no drive of kin selection for cooperation. We have now heard, from bird species to humans, you get the optimal fertility somewhere around third to fourth cousins. You get the optimal fertility, under circumstances that select for a fair degree of homogamy across different cultures. And historically, this is no surprise at all-- that there is a great deal of homogamy. Various studies have shown that your average traditional hunter-gatherer winds up being married to somebody who grew up less than 40 kilometers away from them. Studies show that people in traditional agricultural villages in the developing world wind up being married to someone, on the average, less than 10 kilometers away from them. You are getting an awful lot of people winding up being married with individuals who look a lot like them. And back to that issue of partial relatedness-- third to fourth cousins. Yes. But couldn't it just be that I know a lot more people who are the same age as me, the same race, the same socioeconomic status, and so on? OK, well, though, look around the room. This is a very heterogeneous corner of the planet we've come up with, here. Yes, it's very different when you look in more homogeneous societies, there. But when you begin to look at the increasingly diverse Western European ones, you see a fair amount of homogamy going on. Very interesting data. Iceland-- yet another of our good-old Scandinavian-- is Iceland technically Scandinavian? Or are they just, like, good guys who should be Scandinavians? They're so dull and healthy and sensible. OK. So in those sensible, non-Scandinavian Icelandics, who have, like-- I don't know, what? --300,000 people in the whole country, and all of them are no more than sixth cousins apart, and all of them have just fanatically clean records, going back centuries, as to who was married to who, studies have now been done, looking over the course of 200 years, in Iceland of how closely related different couples were. And what you see is, optimal fertility, optimal number of children who survived into adulthood, you get from third- to fourth-cousin marriages. Recent studies showing that. One additional, interesting version of this homogamy stuff, which is, in the United States you see something slightly different. You see an age factor coming in here, which is, people are more likely to make less homogamous choices as to their mates, the younger they are when they get married. What's that a surrogate for? The incredibly depressing fact that, on the average, people get more and more closed-minded as they get older. You see the greatest heterogamy in marriages-- the greatest likelihood of people marrying someone with a very different background, the younger they are. In the early 20s is when you find the peak for a lot of these traits-- when you find the least homogamy. One interesting exception to that, or a partial exception, which is with religion. And what you see is, you look at people getting married in their early 20s. This is the likelihood of marrying somebody from a different religion. And then it goes way down in the 30s and 40s. And somewhere between 50 and 60, there's another little blip that goes up. And this has been documented in a number of studies. What's that blip about? Any theories? Oh-- OK, somebody [INAUDIBLE] there. Um-- fewer options for mates? Fewer options for mates. That's one depressing possibility. And just wait until you're in the nursing home and, like, anything that moves you imprint on. Yes, theory? They're not going to have kids anymore, so they don't need to worry about passing down [INAUDIBLE] to their kids. OK. So the issue of-- if you do that, if you have waited long enough that you're not going to have kids, that you're not going to have all these fights of, are you going to be a, you know, a Mac family or a PC family or all those sorts of things that can tear people apart [INAUDIBLE]. OK, so that's a possibility. How about another one? Yeah. You're reflecting your mortality. OK, you're reflecting on mortality, and you're deciding, enough of that nonsense. Who cares? This is someone who matters. Yes, that's a possibility. What else? Was that an idea? Midlife crisis? Midlife crisis. So it's either you get a convertible or you marry someone from, you know, Tierra Del Fuego or something. So all of those seem to go into it. One additional one, which I find to be absolutely, like, wonderful, in terms of just how bizarre it is. Which is, when they interview people, what you also see is, the ones who tend to have this peak, they have been in long-term relationships with the individual. What was that about? They're waiting for their parents to die, so they don't kill them by marrying this person. [LAUGHTER] That appears to be part of that scenario. Which is, once they're dead, then we can go get married. Because I couldn't possibly do this to them before then. And that appears to be part of this peak, also. OK. Finally-- finally, a study-- famous study that was done about 15 years ago, by an evolutionary biologist named David Buss, who's now at the University of Texas. He wasn't, at the time. And he did one of these sorts of studies that we recognize by now, which is sort of questioning people, questionnaires of people from a lot of different cultures, all over the planet. And this was this massive study of I don't remember how many different cultures. But it ranged from non-westernized, nomadic pastoralists, non-westernized agriculturalists, socialist countries, communist ones, capitalist ones-- everything in between-- individualistic societies, communalist societies-- all of that. It was tens of thousands of people in the study. Where he gave all these people a list of traits. And he would say, which of these traits are most important to you, in terms of who you marry? And what came out in every single culture looked at-- as follows. In every culture looked at, women are more likely than men to want a mate who is older than them. In every culture looked at, men, more common than women, looking for someone who's younger than them. In every culture looked at, women more than men citing the economic prowess of this individual as being part of what constitutes the desirable traits. In every one of them that's been looked at, men having a greater preference than women for markers-- health markers-- of fertility in the person they would wind up with. This was so depressing, because this is every culture on this planet coming up with this cliche. Everybody learned about the study, and how remarkably disturbing it was in terms of all of those stereotypes across so many different cultures. Women are looking for older guys who have lots of money, and men are looking for younger women with big hips or who knows what. And this came through in every single culture. But what everybody always misses when they discuss this study was one additional one. And this is, like, enough for, like, Hallmark cards-- all of that. What everybody knows is, across these cultures, these traits were more likely to be rated highly by one sex versus the other, but in every single one of the countries, what you find is both sexes had an equal preference for the number one thing on the list, which is winding up with somebody who's nice to them. OK. On that note, let's take a break. We now switch to our next second half of the course topic, which is going to follow the exact same strategy. And where we're shifting to, here, is going to be a whole bunch of lectures on the large, sprawling, interconnected subjects of aggression, violence, competition, cooperation, empathy. And what we will be doing there is wrestling with all sorts of features of the complexities of these and where the underlying biology is-- enormously important social implications, in a lot of these cases. And, of course, what we need to start off with is, what's the behavior about? How objectively do you describe these things? And it struck me-- a good way of starting that, not only giving a sense of the range of what aggression can be like, but also a very important point that's going to come through in this entire lecture-- it struck me that a good thing to be would be to describe a relatively recent exposure I had to human aggression. Which was me being aggressive to somebody else. This was, now, a couple of years ago, before my back disintegrated on me. I used to play soccer two, three times a week. And I had been doing this for decades. And I'm actually totally lousy at soccer. And, like, I'm short and I'm old and I'm not particularly coordinated. And [INAUDIBLE]. So this has never been something that was, like, one of the things I was going to go to the Olympics with. So, a number of years ago, there was consistently this one guy who seemed to always wind up on the other side, and who somehow always wound up guarding me, who was a total son of a bitch. This guy, he was-- he was a lot younger than me, and he was a lot more athletic, and he was a lot taller than me, and he was a lot more skillful in this game. And, on top of everything else, he was a really dirty player. And this was driving me crazy. Every time the ball would get near me, he would elbow me in my ear, and he would do something like-- [LAUGHTER] --that he wasn't supposed to be doing. That sort. And one day, we were out playing, and somebody passed the ball to me. And, as per usual, he effortlessly knocked me out of the way-- crudely and illegally, which no one seemed to notice-- and then stole the ball. And I was pissed. And this guy quickly passed it to someone else. And the two of us were sort of running towards a corner of the field. And momentum was carrying us. And I sort of realized that the ball was now kind of elsewhere in the field, and thus everybody's attention was kind of elsewhere in the field. So we're running along, and I'm, like, a half step behind him, and I just stick my foot in front of his ankle and send him rolling and-- [LAUGHTER AND SCATTERED APPLAUSE] --sprawling, and this was the best-- heaven. This was during the summer. And it turned out there was a mud hole there. It hadn't rained in six months. This was wonderful! So he goes-- and I didn't even go through the pretense of, oh, are you OK? Sorry about that. It was just-- yeah! [LAUGHTER] I had to restrain myself from ripping his stomach open with my canines, at that point. So I felt wonderful! This was, like, the best thing that had happened to me in months. [LAUGHTER] Nelson Mandela. [LAUGHTER] Nelson Mandela. Now in his 92nd year, frail-- frail. An icon of all that is good and wonderful about humans. This man on his last legs, now. What if I had been playing soccer with Nelson Mandela and I did that to him? You'd be horrified! Nobody would be on the edge of applauding that sort of thing. This would be unspeakable, if I had done that to delightful Nelson Mandela at age 92. So what we see here is a very important thing that is going to run through all of this topic. Which is, very little about the social, the environmental, the learning aspects of aggression have to do with learning how to be aggressive. It's all about when. It's all about the appropriate social context for it. Because that speaks to something that is absolutely clear for 99.9% of us. We do not want a world without violence. We love violence. We get excited by it. We will pay good money to see the right displays of competitive violence. We will barely restrain ourselves from leaping in and joining it. It is all so wonderful and exciting. We love violence-- when it's the right kind. When it's in the right context. And a huge percentage of wrestling with this is built around the fact that, in some settings, the exact same behavior gets you medals, gets you promotions, gets you differential reproductive success. And the exact same patterns of what you do with your muscles in another setting is some of the worst things you could do as a human to another human. And it's the same behavior. And what we're going to be wrestling with, over and over and over, in the subject, as we march back, here, is this whole issue of, very little about the biology of violence, competition, all of that, is about how to do the behaviors. It is all about appropriate context. And what we will see is, all sorts of realms of neurological diseases, where things go majorly wrong, in terms of control against violence-- very rarely will it be that it's the magnitude of violence which is wrong. What you see is, it's in the wrong context. Because if it's done right we love it, and we elect people who have been good at it, and we differentially mate with them, and we pay to see it. It's got to be in the right setting. There are very few of us who viscerally are truly, truly pacifists, all across the board. We just don't like violence in certain settings. So that will be part of the challenge, here-- making sense of the biology of social context. And we've already gotten some hints about that in the last topic. So, of course, what we have to start off with is, like, definitions. And it's never more in this realm that you get the "I don't know how to define it, but I know it when I see it" sort of notion in dealing with terms for aggression and violence. Probably a great starting point is, just as with the sexual-behavior subject, starting off with, well, what aspects of violence and aggression are unique to humans? And what the theme has been there, for decades now, is all sorts of domains that used to be thought to be unique to humans no longer are. We've already heard some of these examples, way back in the sociobiology lectures-- all of those wildlife films where, somewhere in there, somebody with a very deep voice has to intone about how "only man kills for pleasure"-- while watching some beasts throwing the old wildebeest into the river, there, and misinterpreting it. And that was always the theme-- we're the only species that kills. And that went down the tubes, way back when, as soon as people started seeing competitive infanticide. That was first seen-- again, I think I mentioned-- in langur monkeys. A primatologist named Sara Hrdy, who was at Harvard at the time-- and she was the first to report this. And-- this could not be! That male langur monkeys were killing babies? This was impossible. And the interpretation was that this was a psychopathology. This was because of increasing habitat degradation, human populations getting closer, crowding them in. These were populations of langur monkeys in some urban areas in India, where she was studying them. This is not normal, because no other species kills. And now that it's up to 20, 25 different species or so that does competitive infanticide, it is quite clear we are not the only species that kills. And we are not the only species that kills in some premeditated, strategic, Machiavellian, good for numbers of copies of our genes kind of way. We are not the only ones-- Jane Goodall by now having documented lots and lots of cases of murder between chimps, females killing each other's babies, males killing other males quite frequently. And, once again, we are not the only species. What's also become clear from more recent work with chimpanzees is, we're not the only species that makes weapons. Chimps have now been observed to take large, heavy branches and break off the ancillary branches, there, and smooth it out and use it as a weapon, to try to hit another animal. This is tool use and tool production. This is another species making a weapon. We are also not the only species that has organized violence. And this is back, again, to chimps. Something mentioned way back when in the lectures-- the facts that chimps are female-exogamous, that business that, at puberty, it's the females who pick up and move to a different group. All of the adult males in a chimp group are relatively related to each other. They are relatives. And what you get, then, is cooperative aggression among males from a particular group. What you will see is border patrols. Goodall was the first to use this term to describe it. You will get-- the males of the group will get into an extremely agitated state with each other-- a state of emotional contagion, where they build up this very high level of excitation. And they then go and patrol the territory between their group and the next group over. And what Goodall was the first to document was, if they encounter a male from the other group, they will kill him. And what she also documented was cases of groups of male chimps systematically killing all of the males of the neighboring group. What is it that we've just seen, now, in another species? Genocide-- the notion of killing an individual not because of who they are but because of what group, what population, they belong to, as part of a desire to eradicate a population as a whole. We are not the only species that has something resembling genocide, if it's termed that way. So where is a domain where we might be unique? Lots of people still argue that humans are the only species that psychopathologically confuse sexual behavior with aggressive behavior-- world of sadism and masochism and all of that. That appears to be something resembling a human-unique trait. OK. Now flipping to the more cheerful side of things-- the cooperation, the empathy stuff-- what aspects of those behaviors are unique to humans? And what people used to think was an exclusively human ability-- yeah? Are there any instances where certain troops of monkeys will not only just kill all the males in a different troop but kill all females and infants, too, as in [INAUDIBLE] genocide? Yeah. They will kill the infants, out of competitive infanticide stuff. And they will then happily hang with the females. And do more than that, with any luck. In terms of-- oh, a very familiar historical strategy with humans, seen again and again. Good end of things. Used to be the rule that the humans were the only species that showed reconciliation-- that showed increased likelihood of affiliative behavior between two individuals after they have had an aggressive interaction. In the aftermath of it, increased odds of doing something affiliative-- making up-- reconciling. Doing something along those lines. And what has emerged in the last 20 years or so is a huge literature showing reconciliative behavior in a couple of dozen other species. First person to report this-- primatologist Frans de Waal, first reporting this in rhesus monkeys, I believe. But lots of other species since then, including dolphins, including whales. And what you see is-- in the aftermath of a fight, you see two individuals are more likely-- in gorillas, for example, to do social grooming in the aftermath-- than at any other time. An increased rate of that happening-- reconciliation. What's remarkable is some of the subtleties in it. And this was work that was done by Marina Cords, at Columbia. And what she showed was, the odds of reconciliation increase when it's a more important, valuable relationship that you have. How was she showing this? These were studies with macaque monkeys. And what she did was set up circumstances-- these were animals that were caged. And there were circumstances where she put food on a tray outside that could be reached for. Where, in one setting, an animal could get the food in all on its own. And in another setting, the only way to get the food tray close enough to the cage was if both of them cooperated. And this was what they were doing habitually. So what's the difference, there? In the second case, you have formed this cooperative relationship with this other individual. You need them, and they need you to pull off this getting the food close to the cage business. What she showed was significantly higher rates of reconciliation between pairs that have a history of cooperating. What could that be interpreted as? More of a game-theory history of cooperation behind you-- more willingness to forgive. Another way of framing it, as she does in her work, is, this is a more valuable relationship that you don't want to screw up. You are more willing to do something reconciliative afterward. You can see, in baboons, reconciling behavior in females. No male baboons ever reconcile. Showing gender differences, there. In bonobos, you see reconciliation is different from all these other species, where, in all the other ones, it's built around social grooming or chimp hugs or whatever. There, of course, as you guessed it, it's sex. Because with the bonobos, anything that happens, and it's time to have sex. But an interesting thing, in terms of this picture of bonobos being this incredibly peaceful species out there in this commune and all of that-- you can't have reconciliation unless you have aggression. They do have aggressive interactions. Otherwise, there would be nothing to reconcile afterward. Even the beatific bonobos have a certain degree of aggression-- very high and varied rates, varied abilities, to pull off reconciliative behavior afterward. More things that used to be just about us. Empathy. And a literature now coming out, showing the building blocks of that in other species, as well. First example in chimps, second example in lab rats. Chimp example. Again, this was work done by Frans de Waal. And wonderful study. What he showed were two circumstances where a male chimp would get pummeled. First circumstance. You've got this low-ranking male who goes up and threatens and starts a fight with a higher-ranking male and gets pummeled into the ground. Second circumstance. Low-ranking guy is sitting there, minding his own business. High-ranking guy is in a bad mood and pummels him into the ground. What's the difference? In the first case, this kid started it, by challenging the guy. And in the second case, he was an innocent bystander. And what de Waal showed is, in the half hour after these incidents, the ones who were innocent bystanders were far more likely to be groomed by females there in the group than the ones who had started it. They were able to distinguish between not just that this is an individual who just got pounded but whether it was their fault or not, or whether they were a victim. And considerably more grooming when it was an individual who had been a victim. Something resembling some proto empathy happening, there. Remarkable study, published in Science a couple years ago. A group from McGill. And what they showed was arguing this is something resembling empathy in rats. Here's what they showed in the study. What they had was, they-- let's see, how were they doing that? They would have one rat that was restrained. And rats don't like it. And they would be giving off ultrasonic alarm calls. Now you put a second rat, and you give them a pain-threshold test. Which is to say, you put them on a perfectly cool surface, and you begin to warm it up, and you see at what temperature do they first lift up one of their paws. And, quick, take them off at that point. And what is their temperature threshold for beginning to find this aversive? And what they showed was that rats would have a lowered threshold, would be more sensitive to this pain stimulus, if they were next door to another rat giving off alarm vocalizations. But it was more subtle than that. That would be easily described in terms of, OK, alarm calls, and stressful, and just putting me in a more agitated state. What they showed was, this only worked when it was rats that were cagemates. If it's rats that knew each other, hearing the other rat in distress made you more sensitive to a pain stimulus. If it was a strange rat, it didn't work at all. Some crude version of something resembling empathy, there. So we're not the only species with it. What's clear, though, is we are the only species that could take it to just domains that are utterly unrecognizable. We are the only species that is moved by people on the other side of the planet who have just been in some catastrophe. We are the only species moved by artwork that depicts suffering, by movie characters, by fictional characters in books. We are taken into realms that are unmatched elsewhere. There's this wonderful video. This was an advertisement for Ikea. Has anyone seen this one, where the light gets thrown out? Is there anyone who hasn't seen it? OK, this is-- opens up. It's this stormy, like, dismal, drizzling night, there. And it's clearly freezing. And you see-- coming into an apartment, you see an old lamp sitting there, with sort of a neck-type lamp thing. And it's sitting there. And suddenly this person appears, picks it up, and walks outside into the rain and puts it down next to the garbage can. Oh! It's being thrown out. And now, thanks to brilliant photography and angles that are shot just underneath the lamp, there, you see it leaning there, looking miserable in the rain and all alone with its light thing, there. And you see, from this angle in the background, there's the apartment, where it's warm-- lights, and all of that. And then you see the most heartbreaking thing. You see the person come by the window, there-- the armchair-- and put down a new lamp-- a new lamp that's nicer. And they even show the person-- just to really get the edge, there-- briefly caressing the new lamp. And then you get a shot back out again in the dark, and it's pouring, and it's drizzling out. And you're sitting there, feeling terrible for this damned lamp, there. How does this work? And then, just to sort of show it, suddenly this person appears in the screen and says, in effect, what's wrong with you? It's a lamp. It's a lamp, and the new one works better. And then on comes "Ikea," which I suspect is not actually a good strategy, because you think of them as heartless individuals who advocate the abandonment of child lamps. But, like, no hippo would know what the deal is, while you're sitting there feeling sorry for this piece of metal, there. We have empathy-- not the only species, but in very distinct and unique realms. What else? We are ostensibly the only species with a sense of justice. Again, that's not necessarily the case. Again, work by Frans de Waal, whose name keeps coming up, who is one of the best, most creative primatologists in the universe. This is work, now, where, again, chimps-- two chimps-- where circumstances were, either it takes one of them to pull a tray of food over, or both of them to cooperate in order to get the food. And what he shows is, after the cooperative relationship has been established with them, if you change the workings of it that it requires both of them to pull the food in but the food winds up going to only one of the chimps, that champ is more likely to share the food with the other individual than if they didn't have a working-together relationship. That chimp, on some Frans de Waal sort of level, was feeling bad for the other guy getting a rotten deal. Under those circumstances, if it was a chimp that you already had a cooperative relationship with, you were more likely to share with them after they had gotten something aversive occur to them. What else? What else, in terms of making sense of other species-- what are unique, what are not? Dominance hierarchies. And if you come from a certain school of sociology, humans are the least hierarchical species out there. We are not territorial, in the sense of lots of other species. We do not have strict hierarchies. And what you see in other species are hierarchies can mean very different things. And the broad dichotomy that's made is between top-down hierarchies and bottom-up ones. Top-down, you have a single, dominating, most aggressive individual who is up there on top. And the characteristic of this-- it's also called a "despotic" hierarchy-- is extremely unequal distribution of resources enforced by violence or threats of violence from above. That's baboons. That's chimps. That's rhesus monkeys. Then you look at vervet monkeys, and you see a bottom-up, what is also termed "egalitarian," hierarchy. There is a hierarchy, but the number one individual there is only there with the cooperation of everyone else. If that individual becomes abusive, they are overthrown. So hierarchy does not, in other species, automatically mean abusive, aggressive, dominating, unequal-distribution stuff. What it is is, it varies by species. What also becomes clear, by the time you're looking at other species, is it gets really hard to figure out what does indeed count as violence. Example-- East African primate called a "patas" monkey. Go and study patas monkeys, and spend 40, 50, 60 years doing it, and what you will see are virtually no instances of male patas monkeys having fights with each other. Wow! That's a pretty unaggressive species. That's kind of nice. Put two male patas monkeys in a cage, and not only will they start fighting soon afterward, they will fight to the death. Because they have no signals of terminating aggression. What's the deal with that? This is a species where the level of male-male tension and male-male aggression is so high that the entire social structure of these species are built around keeping males as far apart as possible. You don't see male patas monkeys having fights, not because they're not aggressive but because they live in these dispersed patterns. So is this an aggressive species, or not? This isn't an aggressive species; you never see a fight. This is an insanely aggressive species; the central feature of their social structure is keeping violent males away from each other. What does it count as? What does it count as when you see an animal leap on top of another animal and rip it to shreds? Sometimes that's aggression, sometimes that's getting dinner. Once again, as with the sexual-behavior realm-- this whole realm, back to the limbic anatomy of, are you looking at aggression? Are you looking at predation? These are totally different biological phenomena. That takes some work. What has always been one of the big questions in the field is, so, what's the deal with rough-and-tumble play? What's play about? Because you see it in endless species out there. A lot of studies show that during periods of famine, for example, one of the last behaviors that disappears from kids is play. It is really, really hard-wired in there. So when you see aggressive play, is this truly aggression, or is this practicing the building blocks for the real stuff that will come later on? What a lot of the primate studies suggest is, it's not practice, it's not play, it's already establishing some of the asymmetries that will be there later in life. OK. So, all sorts of ways in which aggression can pop up in other species, in unexpected realms, in which cooperation, all sorts of things like that-- what are some of the unique human ones? Nonetheless, amid all those similarities, we do things that are completely unprecedented. We are perfectly capable of being as violent as a chimp, when it comes to cudgeling somebody over the head. But we're the only species that could be violent by doing nothing more physically taxing then pulling a trigger or looking the other away or releasing a bomb from 30,000 feet up in the air or being passive-aggressive or damning with faint praise. And we're suddenly doing all sorts of much more subtle things with aggression. Here's three examples of human aggression-- and I've heard about-- which shows just how complex of a phenomenon this could be. First one. This was the child of a friend of mine, when she was about five years old and she was in kindergarten or pre-K or something. And there was another kid there that she was not getting along with. And this was around Easter, and they were painting Easter eggs. And some tussle came up between them. And my friend's daughter broke the other kid's Easter egg. Tears, hysteria, teachers swooping in to say, you're not a bad child but you've done a bad thing, and you can't do stuff like that. And you are going to paint a new Easter egg, to give to her, to make up for the one that you broke. So my friend's kid proceeds to go to the corner with this new egg and some paintbrushes. And what becomes apparent after a while is she's sort of, like, looking over her shoulder and working away on something, here, and glowering back at everybody. And finally she comes up to the other kid and says, here's your stupid Easter egg, Happy Easter, and gives her an egg that she's painted completely black. [LAUGHTER] OK. What's with the aggression, here? Easter eggs-- pastel colors, bunny rabbits-- all that sort of thing. Easter eggs are not supposed to be one solid color and certainly not jet black. That's not what-- what was she doing, here? She was cooperating with the letter of the law while doing as much violence to the spirit of the law as possible. What she was saying was, she is making me paint this egg for you, and I don't like you one bit more than 30 minutes ago. And, showing that the other kid fully understood what this human bit of passive-aggression was about, she burst into tears as soon as she saw the egg. Next example of the subtlety of human aggression. And this one involved my wife. OK. [LAUGHTER] So this was-- occasion. We were, like, driving around somewhere-- minivan, with our kids. And some total jerk, like, cut us off. And-- you know, he could have killed us and our kids, and-- [GROWL OF FRUSTRATION]. My wife was driving. And we sort of get past what should logically have been about five seconds of cursing the person. And she suddenly says, I'm going after this guy. [LAUGHTER] And she proceeds to trail the guy and trails him for about two miles, while I'm sitting there getting increasingly distressed and panicked. And eventually he, like, realizes he's being followed now and is taking sort of evasive maneuvers. And eventually we get him on this street where there's a red light, there, where there's a car in front of him. And then we're behind him. So it's not like he could, in a panic, go through the red light, there, because he's trapped there. And we happen to know this was a very long red light. While I've been sitting there, for the last five minutes, saying, um, do you think this is really a good-- [VROOM] go around another corner, tracking him down. So we're sitting there. And suddenly my wife says, I'm going over there. And she grabs something from between the seats and storms out. While I say, um, do you really think that's a good-- [LAUGHTER] She's gone. So I, like, get out, and I run over there. And I see the window's down in the car. And she's yelling at the guy. And she says, anybody who could do something like that needs this. And she flings something at him. So she comes back to the car, at that point. And the light has changed. And this guy, like, slinks off into the-- if it is possible for a car to look sheepish, moving, like, four miles an hour, heads off into the sunset, down this dark, little block, there. So, sitting there. And she's milking this for all it's worth. And turn-- what did you put in there? And she looks totally delighted with herself. And she's, like, euphoric. And I said, well, what did you say to him? You-- what was that-- and she said, anyone that could do anything this mean needs one of these. And I said, what did you do then? And she said, I threw a lollipop at him. [LAUGHTER] I said, whoa! You killer, you! You-- I was so proud of her. My god, the violence intrinsic in that! No other species would know what was that going on, there, in terms of the intrinsic passive-aggression and all of that. We're the only ones who could come up with something like that. Third example. Every day, out in Nevada, in a town there, there are men who get up to go off to work, and they kiss the family goodbye. And they've got to get reminded to pick up the dry cleaning. And they get in the car, and they're a little bit late, and there's a lot of traffic, and they get all stressed about the traffic jam. But they get there to work on time. And they're a little bit relaxed there. And they finally can come in. And they sit down in a chair in what is a model of the cockpit of a fighter plane. And what they do is control drone airplanes on the other side of the planet, in Iraq. And they spend the day, sitting there at their work shift, controlling planes that release bombs and missiles and destroy people 12,000 miles away. So they spend the entire day, sitting there in this air-conditioned room in Nevis Air Force Base, just outside Las Vegas. And they spend the day doing that. And at the end of the day, they pick up, and they tell everyone they'll see them tomorrow. And they go pick up the dry cleaning. And they go to their little daughter's ballet concert. And afterward they hug her and can't believe they could love somebody this much. And then the next morning they go back to spending their day killing people on the other side of the planet. There is not a whole lot of species out there who could do that, either. So by the time you're getting to us, we have ways of being awful to other members of our species that are simply unmatched. And we have ways of being empathic, as well. As we begin to wrestle, here, with the neurobiology and endocrinology and finally walking our way back towards the left, we're going to have this huge problem of the context of aggression and this even huger problem of just how complex aggression and empathy are, when you leave it to humans. Examples at the empathy end. Human versions of it. The things we are able to reconcile. We are a species who has invented truth-and-reconciliation commissions in South Africa, in the Balkans, in Rwanda, where people face the person who did that to them, the person who destroyed their life, destroyed their family. And going through what is by now a fairly well-worked-out process that all sorts of people have studied. And, in some of those cases, there is reconciliation. There is even forgiveness. How can this possibly happen? We have a world where we try to have individuals foster peace through the most unlikely of rationales. I will let no man spoil my soul by causing me to hate him. That is a psychology that is unprecedented. We have the world of people like this Catholic nun, Sister Helen Prejean, who has spent her entire career ministering to the needs of men on death row in a maximum-security prison in Louisiana. She was the person who was featured in the movie way back when, Dead Man Walking. And what she spent a lot of her time doing is having incredulous people, often the relatives of the victims of these murderers, come up and say, how can you do this? How could you spend your life devoted to people like these? And she comes up with an answer that is so definedly human, that no other species could come near. No matter how much they groom victims, her answer always is, the less forgivable the act, the more it must be forgiven. The less lovable the person, the more they must be loved. And suddenly we're in a world that the more something cannot be, the more it must be, as a moral sort of act to do. Whoa-- nobody out there in the animal kingdom is going to have a clue what we're up to. This is very complex terrain we are going to deal with, here, as we now shift over to the biology of it. OK. So, starting now with our strategy-- a violent act occurs. An empathic act occurs. Any in these categories. What was going on in the brain, one second before? And, of course, what do you know? We're going to immediately going to land is, right in the middle of the limbic system. And, just as the Kluver-Bucy syndrome from the 1980s, as we heard about last week-- just as that syndrome, when you destroy the limbic system, you get completely inappropriate sexual behavior in primates. What those studies also showed was, you get completely inappropriate aggressive behavior. OK. So, with 80-year-old research, we've now landed in the limbic system. What are the subregions that are relevant? The area that comes in at the top of the list, immediately, that we've already heard a fair amount about, is the amygdala. The amygdala and its role in fear, its role in anxiety-- that strange role, in males, of sexual motivation. But what the amygdala is most renowned for is its role in aggression. And, as I emphasized last week, it is mighty interesting, I think, that the part of the brain which is most responsive to when you are scared is the part of the brain that generates the starts of aggressive behavior. Again, in a world in which no neuron need be afraid, we're going to have a lot fewer aggressive amygdalas out there. What's the evidence? You know the drill by now. You go and you destroy the amygdala in an animal, and you are incapable of eliciting aggression from them. You go and you do the same thing in a human, and you get the exact same result. One of those dark, horrible chapters in the history of science-- another realm of legally enforced psychosurgery. This was a trend that was very, very popular in the '60s and '70s-- court-ordered amygdalectomies of people. And this was a technique where people would go in with a lesioning syringe into each side-- the amygdala is a bilateral structure-- there's two of them, way deep in the brain-- and go in and destroy the structure. And this would decrease aggression. This would decrease all sorts of stuff. This wound up on the front page of the New York Times when, around 1970, three neurosurgeons at Harvard wrote a letter to the New York Times, pointing out there's this great surgical technique which they were the pioneers of where you could take aggressive humans and make them less aggressive with no other side effects. You could make them less aggressive. And haven't you been noticing that our inner cities have been burning and rife with violence? Maybe it's time to start thinking about doing some of these preemptively. This was a letter from these three guys from Harvard Med School. No surprise, not everybody sort of thought this was the swellest idea that they had ever heard. This generated this huge fight over the psychosurgical use of court-enforced amygdalectomies. And before it was over with, it had just as bad of a history as frontal lobotomies. People subjected to amygdalectomies because they were argumentative, because they, as teenagers, didn't listen to their parents. They didn't listen to their teachers. Thousands of cases of these. The one thing that was clear was that, yes, indeed, aggressive behavior would decrease in these individuals. There would not be a whole lot of a person left afterward. So, lesioning evidence. Stimulation evidence. You know the flip side of that by now. Now stick an electrode into the rat's amygdala and stimulate, electrically stimulate, there. And you produce wildly aggressive behavior. And you see two equivalents of that in humans. And in both of these cases, stupendously rare. First is a very, very, very rare type of epilepsy, where what you get is the epileptic focus, the place where the seizure begins, is in the amygdala. What you see is, with most types of epilepsy, the place where it starts in the brain, a seizure, tells you a whole lot about matching with the behavior. People, just before the onset of the seizure, will get olfactory auras-- two or three seconds of hallucinating an odor. That's a seizure that's starting somewhere down in the olfactory part of the limbic system. You see all sorts of cases. There have been documented cases of epileptics who see a mathematical equation for two seconds before the seizure hits. And that's an area of the cortex where it turns up. Auditory seizures, where they hear two measures of music-- the same two measures before it hits. What you see when you've got these rare epilepsies, where the seizure begins in the amygdala, is, two seconds before, the person becomes furious. They-- I can't bel-- I am so ang-- and then it happens. So, uncontrolled stimulation in the amygdala, and you get some aggression there. The next thing that you see is evidence of the amygdaloid stimulation. OK. Anybody ever hear of a guy named Charles Whitman? Hands-- oh, what are they teaching you guys, here? OK. Charles Whitman was once America's record-holding mass murderer. And he was the best that we could come up with in the early 1960s. And, oh, his records have been eclipsed by so many people since then. But he was once our gold-medal mass murderer. This was the guy who, in 1962, I think, climbed up the famed clock tower at the University of Texas, Austin, campus and opened fire on people below. And then killed himself, afterward. This was the first of the rounding up the neighbors to say, oh my god, he was such a quiet guy-- he was such a nice neighbor. This was the first of the literal choirboys-- my god, where did this come from-- no hint of anything. On postmortem, he was found to have a tumor in his amygdala. Another rare case of that. During the 1970s, there was an extreme leftist terrorist group in Germany called the Baader-Meinhof gang. And these were the two people who began it. And one of the two stupendously violent people-- one of the two found, on postmortem, to have a tumor in her amygdala. So these very, very rare cases of this, fitting with this theme-- circumstances that increase the metabolism in the amygdala. Out comes aggressive behavior. More evidence. Now, another strategy from the limbic lectures. Put in the electrode. This is one that responds to electrical signaling. Show somebody something that evokes aggression, and their amygdala has gotten activated. Do the same thing, now, with a human. Put them in a brain scanner, and show them something that evokes anger. And the metabolic rate in their amygdala activates. So you see all sorts of circumstances where you can document the amygdala as playing this role. Interestingly, the amygdala gets bigger, as we've heard, in people with posttraumatic stress disorder. And what you see there is also increased frequency of violent behavior. More evidence that the amygdala plays a role-- and this was a very subtle finding. This was from-- oh my god, the clock in the back! Turn around and look at that clock. [LAUGHTER] Everybody run! [LAUGHTER] OK, well, now that it's almost done, I'll mention the very last thing, here. These were studies done showing-- OK, people with amygdaloid lesions, they are very bad at detecting faces expressing angry emotions. They are more trusting of people than average individuals are. They are more likely to forgive. They are less capable of picking up on any of that information. And a wonderful study done by this guy, Antonio Damasio-- again, one of the lead figures in the field. What he did was eye tracking on people with amygdaloid lesions. You get someone with this part of the brain destroyed, and they don't look at the eyes of other people. When they examine faces, they're looking at the nose. They're looking at the chin. They're not directing their gaze to be able to pick up accurate information about emotions. So what do we see, here? The amygdala not only responds to aggression and fear-provoking stimuli. The amygdala is able to direct you to look for it. And what we will see on Wednesday is, one of the things testosterone does in males is it makes them look harder. It lowers their threshold for deciding that ambiguous information is, in fact-- For more, please visit us at stanford.edu.
Info
Channel: Stanford
Views: 372,682
Rating: 4.8429174 out of 5
Keywords: biology, sexual reproduction, aggression, competition, selection, evolution, homosexuality, genetics, reproduction, characteristic, trait, attractiveness, symmetry, secondary, health, fertility, quality, species, infant, mating, pair bonding, courtship
Id: JPYmarGO5jM
Channel Id: undefined
Length: 96min 41sec (5801 seconds)
Published: Tue Feb 01 2011
Related Videos
Note
Please note that this website is currently a work in progress! Lots of interesting data and statistics to come.