11. Introduction to Neuroscience II

Video Statistics and Information

Video
Captions Word Cloud
Reddit Comments
Captions
Stanford University. I would like to get started, please. So my name is Patrick House. I am a neuroscience PhD student. I work with Robert in Robert's lab. I'm a first year. And I study something that you guys will eventually hear about, but I don't want to ruin the punchline. But today we're going to talk about memory and plasticity. And so two days ago on Wednesday, you guys all sat in here, in this room. And you learned, some of you for the first time, some of you for maybe the 10th time, the basics of neurobiology, of how a neuron works, how a neuron-- you have a presynaptic cell, you have a postsynaptic cell, and this kind of simplified version of the communication and information transfer. And something interesting happened between now and then, which is that now you sit in the same room. And something about you knows something about neuroscience now. You heard one of the TAs talk. You slept on it. And then you come back now and you have assimilated, integrated into your identity, into what you know, new facts. And this lecture is about how you do that. And to kind of get at what memory is, we need to think about a lot of different ways in which it's interesting, and a lot of different spectrums and severities about memory. So why is it that some memories last our entire lives, whereas other memories, we hear and they're fleeting? They go away in a second. Why is it that someone sitting next to you in bed, telling you a story, and as you go to sleep you can't remember it? As you wake up, you can't remember your dreams. But if that exact same person, that exact same story was told to you as they were sitting next to you in a car, and you get into a car accident, suddenly that memory becomes salient. You may remember it for years, if not your entire life. And you may actually associate either the story itself, the voice of the person, with that traumatic event. And you might get post-traumatic stress disorder. So if the mechanism is the same between these two types of memory, between ones that are fleeting and forgetful and ones that last your entire life, the question is, how does environment, how does context fit into shaping these types of memories? And so in order to understand that, we have to kind of get at, what are the mechanisms of memory? And how are these contextually motivated? So I want introduce to you first Stephen Wiltshire, who is an architect, if not in practice, at least in mind. He is an autistic savant. And he has been mute since age three. And he has this remarkable capacity, which I'm actually going to test you guys on slightly here, if you have any kind of inclination to sketch, or you happen to have some sketch paper with you. I want you to-- this will take approximately 60 seconds to span across all of Rome-- draw it from memory in your 60 seconds. Because Stephen Wiltshire has this amazing capacity to take helicopter rides-- he's done this over Tokyo. He's done this over New York. He's done this over Rome and over London. And in 20 minutes, he can then sit down and recreate every single building, every single column, every single window in correct proportions, from the correct angle in which the helicopter ride was. And so you may be thinking-- OK, your 60 seconds are almost up. Can you guys do it? You may be thinking-- if any of you are artists out there, you may be thinking this is unfair. Why can't I do this? And as neuroscientists, our first thought is, OK, this is unfair. Why can't I do this? But really, it can tell us something interesting about memory. So you come at it with two questions. First question-- before and after this helicopter ride, what is different in Stephen's brain? And second, on this theme of individual variation that we keep harping on in class, why is it that he can do this and we can't do this? And these are two important questions that if we could answer those questions, we would know a lot about what memory is. And so it really makes sense to go back to what it is that we know so far about neurons, the basics of one neuron and how it is activated. And so we have a presynaptic cell, and we have a postsynaptic cell. And in our simplified version, we can kind of know now-- and what I'm going to tell you is that memory, learning happens, to the best of our knowledge, in the synapse, in the space between the pre and the postsynaptic cell. But to understand why it is that we think that, we kind of need to go back about 100 years to when people, scientists, neuroscientists were investigating the brain, investigating memory. And they thought that the smallest unit of the brain that they knew was the neuron. So because of our tendency to explain what we don't know in terms are the smallest unit of thing that we do know, they thought OK, this makes sense-- a memory is a new neuron. And when you learn a new fact, when you learn the basics of neuroscience, you are growing new neurons. And each individual fact is associated with one new neuron. For instance, they may have thought that OK, you guys learned on Wednesday that the axon hillock is the site of the generation of the action potential. So then that is a new fact, and then a new neuron would then be formed. I just realized that that actually might not make sense, because at the time that they thought that, they didn't know what an axon hillock is, so maybe that formulation doesn't even make sense. But the idea is that a couple decades later, people discovered the synapse. They discovered that neurons were not just one interconnected thing, that there is space. There is a gap between them. And as soon as they discovered the synapse, that then became the smallest bit of information we knew about the brain. And then theories came out saying, well, no, memory must be the formation of synapses. So the dogma at the time was then that OK, new fact, axon hillock. What does this mean in the brain? You can see this in the brain. This takes the shape of a new synapse being formed. And what we think of now is that well, this isn't exactly right, because new synapses are not being formed all the time. And new neurons are not formed in the adult brain, which isn't entirely true, but we'll get back to that. But the idea is just that memory and storage of learning, what I'm going to tell you is that it's in the synapse, and that it involves modulation and change of the synapse. And why do we think that? Because we understand the molecules. And we understand at a molecular level what's happening in a synapse when it changes. So that it is thus now our smallest level of understanding of the brain. And so of course, we think oh, well that's probably where memory is. So that's the dogma that we're going to start with. And we're going to start with this idea that memory is synaptic plasticity. Memory is when the space between the presynaptic and postsynaptic neuron changes in some way. And not only that, it changes in one direction. It gets stronger. It's strengthened. And so what this means is that if you have your presynaptic neuron and you fire it, and you get some amount of response, that over time, if you give enough presynaptic activation in a certain time window, that you will then get a heightened, strengthened response in your postsynaptic cell eventually. And that is the kind of mechanism, the overarching, broad mechanism of LTP. And so what we need to do to understand memory is to focus on the synapse. So what we get is our classical picture, which is that you have your synapse. And a neurotransmitter is coming out. And that neurotransmitter is excitatory. And in your postsynaptic cell, what you're getting is you're getting a small amount of activation. You're getting current that comes into that cell. You're getting ions, some sort of response for any individual piece of neurotransmitter. So what this is, is a version, a simplified version, of what is called Hebbian plasticity. And so there's this guy Hebb, which you have to know. There's a few of names you have to know in neuroscience, and he's one of them. And Hebb came up with this idea, the only bumper sticker that neuroscientists ever have on their car, which is that neurons that fire together wire together. And what this is saying is that you have your standard picture of a very, very simplified version of a presynaptic cell that's releasing an excitatory neurotransmitter, and that when it does so, you get a response in your postsynaptic cell. So if you will remember from one lecture ago, that excitatory neurotransmitter is glutamate, though if you're going to spend any time and energy into remembering one neurotransmitter that might be relevant for the class, this is the one you want to remember. You don't have to remember it. So mostly, the idea is just that it's excitatory. And why is this important? Because information is transferred in the brain through activation. And in order to transfer information, you need excitatory neurotransmitters. You need your neurons to be activated. But as we know from what I've told you so far, that repetition is what drives memory, I would suggest that you remember that glutamate is the excitatory, and one and only neurotransmitter that you have to know. Say there's a test question that says, what is the one excitatory neurotransmitter in the brain that you have to know? You would respond glutamate. And then you would be right. And so you can imagine that for this type of simplified diagram, if we were to strengthen the synapse, if we were to get some sort of plasticity, some sort of change, potentiation, you could think of a few ways of doing this. You could take the presynaptic neuron and change how much excitatory neurotransmitter is released. You could change how much glutamate is released. And presumably, if you release more of the little circles with positive charge in them, then you're going to get more activation in the postsynaptic cell. Another thing you could do is basic neurochemistry in this diagram, is that each of those neurotransmitters is binding to a receptor on the postsynaptic cell. And so what you can do is take that individual receptor. And you can make it respond more to a single, individual unit quanta of neurotransmitter. Alternately, you could just increase the number of postsynaptic receptors on your cell. And all of these would be mechanisms by which you could take this very, very simple synapse and potentiate it, such that you get the same release, you get the same neurotransmitter. And what you get is subtle, because LTP is often very subtle. You'll just notice that the response is slightly larger, a slightly larger response given the same amount of input, given the same amount of output of the presynaptic cell. And this is the entire idea of LTP, long-term potentiation, this idea that at your individual synapse, you can potentiate it. You can change it. It is plastic. But there should be one large red flag here, which is-- one of these caveats is, well, your presynaptic neuron, how does it know whether or not LTP should be undergone? How does it know whether or not LTP has been induced? This requires a kind of communication between both the pre and the postsynaptic cell, that how would the presynaptic cell, which has already released its neurotransmitter, know? And what you get is this kind of heretical type of neurotransmitter that can actually-- we call it a retrograde neurotransmitter-- that can actually be sent back from the postsynaptic cell. And it's a gas. Nitric oxide is an example of one. It's what you get at your dentist. And it kind of goes back and diffuses back across the synapse and actually modulates how much neurotransmitter gets released from the presynaptic cell. So we have these mechanisms. We have these mechanisms of LTP. And the question then is, where does this happen in the brain? And why is it that we believe that these are the places of LTP? And one of the things you need to know-- this is our first dive into neuroanatomy-- is you need to know the hippocampus. It was introduced to you last lecture. But if there's one neurotransmitter you need to know, it's glutamate. And if there's one neural anatomical structure you need to know at this point, it's the hippocampus. I'm kind of always jealous of these autistic savants that can memorize 10,000 digits of pi, and take a helicopter ride and then fully recreate the cityscape of any city they see. And if you read interviews about how they do it, it's really interesting. So what they seem to do is not memorize just a sheet of a bunch of digits, a string of 10,000 digits of pi. They'll take a walk through their childhood town. And they'll say that they put one of the digits on each and every object. So your mailbox will be the first digit, and then your neighbor's door will be another, and then their window will be three. And then they are not re-conceiving and reconstructing just a sheet of boring numbers. They're taking spatial tours through their memory. And what this always compels me to do is make these kind of visual mnemonics. So I'm going to give you a visual mnemonic for the one way you have to remember that the hippocampus is the site of memory and the site of LTP, insofar as this class is concerned, which is-- OK, hippo horse. We learned that last time, it looks kind of like a seahorse. But that doesn't really make sense. What if you think about this? What if you think about the hippodrome back in Rome? Hippodrome is the circular arena where you had your little chariot races, because of horses. And there's two different scenarios that I want you to imagine. The first is-- you guys remember Michael Jordan and Larry Bird? They had this commercial where they played Horse. And what they were doing is they were playing-- you have to make a basketball shot. And then the next one, you have to remember what that first person did exactly, and you have to recreate it. So what I like to imagine is Larry Bird and Michael Jordan playing a game of horse in the hippodrome, back in Rome. And then you can kind of get this idea of how memory is related to the hippocampus. and this horse structure. And if that doesn't work for you, I have one more, which is actually my favorite, which is you can imagine the entire amphitheatre, the entire hippodrome filled with people. And there was that one emperor who named his horse a senator. Do you guys know who that is? What's his name. I don't know his name. Caligula. Caligula, there you go. So imagine the entire hippodrome is filled with people. And Caligula is there. And he gets his senator horse in the middle of the field. And the horse is sitting kind of cross-legged. And he's typing out your memoirs on a typewriter. And that is how you're going to remember that the hippocampus, the hippodrome, the horse, is where memory is formed. So now you guys are all autistic savants now. Really though, what we need to determine is, why is it that we really think that the hippocampus is the site of LTP and memory formation? It turns out that there is actually adult neurogenesis and adult plasticity. So in the last 10 years, we've discovered that the adult brain really does actually form new neurons. And for the last 100 years, we kind of disregarded that and said those guys who initially believed that every new neuron is associated with every new fact, those guys were just totally wrong and ridiculous. What a ridiculous concept. And so perhaps eventually, in the future, we will have to incorporate this idea that there does seem to be some neurogenesis in the brain. But much like how we learned that there is non-genetic inherited traits, which we had learned from the disreputable Lamarck, way back in Lamarckian evolution, that there is no-- for 100 years, we thought, no, it's impossible. There is no non-genetic inheritance of traits. But it turns out that, well, OK, we do seem to have some kind of non-genetic inheritance. And so Lamarck isn't entirely wrong. And it turns out this is a kind of similar thing, where the people of most disrepute are often just a little bit right. So probably, people that used to think that adult neurogenesis has something to do with memory are probably a little bit right. But we're going to stick with the canon, which is that LTP happens, and that it happens not on the level of the neuron, not on the level of the synapse, but on the level of the plasticity of the synapse. And so why do we think it's the hippocampus? We get at it from a few ways. The first way is that HM, this kind of well-dressed epileptic who had his hippocampi removed. And what happened was he had selective removal of just his hippocampi. And he could no longer remember anything at all. He could not form new memories whatsoever. So with these types of conclusions, in addition to evidence that if you watch and record from neurons in the hippocampus as you're giving someone a learning task, then you see LTP. If you pharmacologically block LTP, you see changes in the hippocampus. And so all these pieces of evidence are trying to get at the idea that the hippocampus is necessary for memory and memory consolidation. But if you introspect a little bit, you can probably realize that well, we undergo all kinds of forms of learning and memory. We have motor learning. We learn how to shoot baskets. We learn how to throw darts. We understand emotionally that events that are more emotionally salient are more memorable. And so how is it that these types of things are also encoded in our brain, also encoded in the same region, the one region, the hippocampus? And what that turns out to be is that, well, it's not just that one region. LTP is happening all over the brain, that if you look in your emotional regulation centers, if you look in your emotional cortices, you also see LTP. And this makes sense, because these types of memories have to have different methods of storage and retrieval. And also, that you can imagine this type of excitation, this type of synaptic plasticity, can go wrong. In post-traumatic stress disorder, for instance, you get LTP. And you get LTP that is severe. And you get severe LTP potentiation of your synapses in those emotion regulation centers that create a situation where the context leads to memories that shouldn't necessarily be brought up, that shouldn't necessarily be retrieved. And so we see this mechanism for types of behavior that we know, types of things like why certain memories, certain emotionally salient memories last for a long time, and others don't. And we can also imagine that this is a physiological process, and that it can go wrong, occasionally. We all know that memories are degraded, sometimes intentionally so, sometimes unintentionally so, that there are certain things we want to remember. And despite any and all repetition about glutamate being the excitatory transmitter, we just don't remember them. And there are some that just kind of fade away into time, into the oceans. And what is happening is that there are mechanisms for intentional disruption of LTP. And you can think of a few. So hypoglycemic states, if you are really, really hungry, you get insulin cascades that end up reducing LTP. If you're starving, it's not a good time to try to remember things. It's a good time to try to go out and expend energy finding food. As we'll learn later in lectures, there are some stress hormones. And these stress hormones actually give us a selective memory advantage in the short term. If you're in a car crash, you remember the slow motion details of the entire event. And this has to do with these stress hormones, these fear hormones coming out and saying, OK, well, we want to be able to remember this moment so we can learn from it next time, if we survive. But if you do this chronically, if you do this for a long time window throughout the lifetime of the organism, then you can actually get damage to LTP and damage to memory. So it's about time window. It's about the same mechanisms that can enhance memory can also be deleterious eventually. Another probably more familiar one, perhaps not to the introverts, but perhaps to the extroverts in the crowd, is that if this lecture were on Saturday morning, I could probably ask you guys what you did last night. And some of you would not be able to tell me with delicate accuracy what happened on Friday. And you might not be able to tell me the story that was read to you at bedtime, or even who read you the story at bedtime. And this is because ethanol, alcohol, directly disrupts LTP. And we see this. And we see this in the hippocampus. And these are the types of things, behaviors, that we know of. We know that emotionally salient memories last longer. We know that alcohol somewhat-- there are differential effects of types of substances on memory. We know that it's hard to remember things right before we go to sleep. And so what's interesting is, can we get at a physiology that explains all of these things? And so I'm going to give you 60 more seconds-- there's going to be a pop quiz at the end, by the way-- you have 60 more seconds to do this. And what is interesting here is that when we get down to these physiological mechanisms, we have two ends of a spectrum. We have HM and we have Stephen Wiltshire, someone who cannot form any memories whatsoever, and then someone who can do this in a 20-minute helicopter ride, recreate the entire landscape. And the question is, the theme of this class is often one of individual variation. How is it that one person can not be able to form any memories whatsoever? How is it that one person can have an autistic, photographic memory? And where do we fit? Where does memory fit in a properly functioning way? And like most of the spectrums that are introduced into this class, one of imprinted genes, tournament versus pair-bonding species, things like that, the answer turns out to be we are somewhere in the middle between HM, no hippocampi, no formation of new memories, and Stephen Wiltshire. So one more thing we need to discuss with the theme of this class is that often, we'll give you a lecture and then maybe in the next lecture, maybe five minutes later, we'll tell you it's all wrong. Or we'll say, no, you've been way too myopic. That's not how you should see these things. And what we need to do in order to understand somewhat about the context of memories is to take and expand your myopic view of this simplified version of a neuron. So far, we've gone into what a single neuron functioning looks like. And we've gone into what a single neuron as it transmits a signal to another neuron looks like, how there's a gap in between the pre and the postsynaptic cell, and what that information transfer looks like, and how we can change that information transfer, how we can make it plastic. But there's a problem here, which is that if we're trying to learn anything about the brain, we have to understand that the brain is really complicated, and that there's 100 billion neurons, and that sometimes these individual neurons will connect to 10,000 other neurons. And sometimes, each of those 10,000 neurons will have 10,000 neurons that connected to it. And so the question-- I don't know. As a neuroscientist, when I look at that, the first thing I do is want to give up. And I do. And then the second thought is OK, maybe it's time to expand the simplified version of the neuron that we have. It's not just one neuron talking to another neuron. It's not just a single synapse, but that it's the dynamics of many, many interacting neurons. And as these dynamics expand, as these dynamics get introduced into 10,000 neurons at the same time, 10,000 dendrites, dendritic arbors connecting to 10,000 other axonal processes, then we see that things that didn't matter so much in the single, individual neuron actually matter quite a bit when you're talking about 100 billion neurons. So one of the things to introduce here is the concept of noise into the individual signal transfer, into the individual information transfer. A neuron, as we presented it, was something that fires an action potential, transmits information. Every single action potential leads to neurotransmitter release, which leads to postsynaptic response. But these are very delicate things. An individual neuron is constantly in flux with how much current is coming in and out. Ions are flowing around. It's not as simple as a static neuron that then gets activated, and then passes on a message to another static neuron, which then gets activated. What you get is often, a lot of times, you'll get random and spontaneous generation of signal, of action potentials, and sometimes of current in the postsynaptic cell. And one of the major tasks of the brain is figuring out what is signal, what is appropriate and meaningful signal, versus what is this noise. If you can imagine on a single neural level, the noise might not have that much impact. But if you're talking about 100 billion neurons, you're going to get noise all over the place that will just lead to this static of noise that you don't know what to make of the world anymore. You don't know what to make of the signal. You don't know what to make of individual neural signals. And so what we need to do is to start considering neurons in terms of how they interact in dynamics of groups. And one of the first ways and the most important ways to think about this is to understand that neurons are not just excitatory forces, that information, yes, is generated by glutamate and the transfer of excitation, but that neurons have a capability to inhibit. And one of the important ways that they differentiate signal from noise, one of the important ways to learn what is noise and what is not, is to inhibit. And I'll explain how it is exactly that the inhibition works. But one of the first-- oh, that's pretty high. One of the first ones to understand is that a neuron can inhibit itself, which is not really-- it seems like it could initially be some sort of masochism, but it's really not. It's just that the neuron is trying to sharpen the signal that it's sending. So a neuron is firing and firing, over and over and over, and what it wants to say, what it wants to be able to do, is accurately give a precise description of the signal, of the information. And what it can do is inhibit itself to say, I'm done. No more spontaneous noise. No more spontaneous little bits of current. I am done with my signal. And what this is, is it allows for temporal sharpening. It allows for the ability of a neuron to say, this was my signal. It was meaningful. I really meant it, and inhibit the kinds of random noise and spontaneous things that could happen. Another type of inhibition that's very important to separate noise from not noise is spatial inhibition. So what this is, is your individual neuron, not only can it send processes and inhibit itself, it can actually send processes out and inhibit its neighbors. And how might this be useful and important is that it can say, essentially, OK, this signal is real. This signal is the signal that I want to send, the information that I want to transfer. And not only that, ignore my neighbors. It's really me. And what this allows you to do is get spatial sharpening. So what this allows you to do is say, in the field of things that you're trying to perceive, a certain neuron will respond to a certain section of that field. And what this is saying is, I am activated. And you inhibit your neighbors so that you're more sure that your signal is true. And how can we relate this? How can we make sense of this? There's a very simple type of feedback network that should elaborate this idea, which is pain and pain sensation. And so we all probably, at some point in our lives, presumably, have discovered and felt pain. And there's two general qualities of pain. You can have really, really fast, sharp pain. And you can have this dull, aching, throbbing pain. And what people found when they investigate into your spinal cord and into your sensory peripheral processes is how this pain is generated. And it's generated on two separate types of neurons. And one carries fast pain, one carries the sharp, fast stuff. One carries the slow, dull stuff. And what you find is that these are intertwined in this delicate feedback loop, such that the fast, spiking, first pain will generate, eventually, the slow-moving pain. It will fire the other neurons next to it, the neighbors, and say, OK, also start this slow pain spike. But then the slow pain spike can come back and inhibit the first sharp spike, such that it stops. We're trying to get information about the world. And your body is trying to do what it can with that information. And if you get stung by something, you want really sharp pain to be like, hey, pay attention to that. Make sure it's not a scorpion that's still there. But you don't need this sharp pain forever. You want to be able to inhibit it and just say, OK, pay attention. But then, just to make sure you don't walk on it anymore and get it infected, we're going to make it hurt a little bit. And so this is your body trying to make the most of this type of information. And what it's doing is using lateral inhibition in this complicated way, actually simple way, to allow for these two types of transmissions of information. That was a very simple example. And I think there's a much more complex example when we go into the types of complex visual stimuli that vision gives us. And you can imagine that lateral inhibition, the same type of spatial sharpening of a signal, can come into play as we're trying to figure out and piece together the visual world. So what this is doing, what this kind of lateral inhibition allows for, is it allows for visual neurons to receive input and then to say, it is me. This is the signal that I want to send. Not only that, inhibit the neighbors. And what this leads to is this emergent property of these retinal cells that allow for specific types of signal and allow for specific types of receptive fields. So in your eyes, your neurons in the back of your eyes, if you just stand still, they only have a certain angle of light that they can get. And that idea is this idea of receptive fields, that they are responsible for that field. And they're responsible for saying, if there's a signal there, this is what it is. And this is how it's relevant. And what this type of lateral inhibition allows you to do is it allows you to say, OK. Your neuron gets a signal, and it wants to say, OK, that's an edge, an edge detection, contrast detection. If you look around at the objects in the room, often you define them by their edges. So we have this elaborated neural mechanism involving inhibition and excitation that allows for this type of contrast enhancement. And what can we do with this even more? So these guys Hubel and Wiesel decided that they were going to look-- OK, another brief anecdote. So there's this commercial when I was young. And it was Michelin Tires. And if any of you guys ever become marketing people, which there's enough of you that statistically, someone will. I don't know why you don't make commercials that are scary, because this commercial frightened me. And I was, like, six. And I remember it to this day. So why not, if you want someone to remember your product, just make it-- take what you know from this, and use it to manipulate people. That's what education is. And so I remember this commercial. And I just remember it was for Michelin Tires. And their whole point was that no matter how fancy your car is, no matter how much you spend on your car, there's only four points of contact between you and the road. And it's on your tires. And I don't know why, but this blew my mind, and it scared me. And it made me recognize that, yeah, you should get good tires. So Hubel and Wiesel, they took essentially the same logic, which is that we have this complicated visual world, and we know that we put it together somehow, but our only access to this information is through the retina. We have the light is the road. These are our two tires. We only have two tires to connect with the road. And so their logic was that if we look at each individual neuron in the retina and trace it back, we should be able to see, somehow, how this visual world is constructed, how it is that we go from the only signal, the only signal from the outside world we get, to this complicated visual field. And what they found was that if you look at the neurons in the back of a retina, and then look at where they synapse, back in your visual cortex, they found a one-to-one correspondence, that if you activate a certain neuron in your retina, you'll get a spatiotopic-- which means they're oriented in the same way, and all the neurons are aligned in a similar way-- field in the back of your visual cortex, in V1. And so what that essentially means is that your eyes are smushed to the back your head. There's no information that necessarily gets enhanced or reduced between your retina and the back of your visual field. And they're like, OK, great. And so these guys, Hubel and Wiesel, won the Nobel Prize. If you could win more than one, they probably would have won four by now. They were these Harvard neuroscientist back in the day. And they are pretty much who you need to know. You need to know Hebb. You need to understand glutamate. You need to understand LTP. And Hubel and Wiesel, if you're going to be a neuroscientist, be interested in the brain, they will come up. And if you're a neuroscientist, you have to invite them to your wedding. You have to do everything. I don't even know if they're-- they might even be dead. You have to seance them, or something. But what they decided to do was then, OK, now we're at V1. We're at the one level of visual cortex. What else can we do? Where does the scene get constructed that we see? And what they did was looked one layer up. And they did the exact same thing. They fired individual retinal neurons. And they looked in the next layer up. And absolutely nothing happened. There was no activity anywhere, no matter what they did. It fired all of them, and there was no activity. And they were like, oh, damn it. We're not going to get invited to any weddings. What are we going to do? But what they discovered was that if you activated enough retinal neurons, and that they were in a certain spatial orientation, say a line, then you get activation in this other layer of your visual cortex. And what they discovered, and what was on, I'm sure, the construction of all of their wedding invitations, was that if you have certain neurons that are selective to certain orientations of lines, like so, if you imagine these are four neurons, a single neuron will be responsive to a vertical line. A single neuron, another one, a different one, will be responsive to a 45-degree angle, a horizontal line, 135 degrees. And so what you get, and you're starting to piece together, is this way of constructing the visual world that is layered, and that extracts out features, and that through those features you get individual neural activation. And you might imagine that if you go up even further, you would get some sort of more higher order types of activation, individual activation, in your visual cortex, something like, say, a neuron that only responds to an orange, or a neuron that only responds to a banana, or a neuron that-- so this is one of the terms in the field-- only responds to your grandmother. And they call it a grandmother neuron. And there's this kind of El Dorado type quest for grandmother neuron. Where can we find it? And the problem is, nobody ever found it. So then the question becomes, again, related to memory, related to even our understanding of these kind of networks of neurons. Where are these memories stored? This is slightly out of order. But basically, this is an example of, again, your lateral inhibition. So to understand how it is that these signals are sharpened, this is a nice visual illusion. Do you guys see little dots of dark between each of those? That is an artifact. That is an artifact of your visual perception. That is an artifact of you constructing that. And why is that happening? Because at each of those individual corners, you're getting the most amount, because of the four axial bars of white, of lateral inhibition. So in every single one of your brains right now, whenever you look at the individual corner, you're getting lateral inhibition. And so you can imagine that this type of thing is a demonstration of lateral inhibition. Another type of thing, when I was talking about pain, there's this fascinating thing where-- we all know this-- where if something itches, say, you have a mosquito bite. And you want to scratch the hell out of it, because sometimes it feels really good. You notice also that you can scratch around it. You can make hard, kind of painful stimuli in the immediate vicinity, and just do it really, really hard enough. And you get lessening of pain. And what that is, is the same type of thing. It's lateral inhibition of the focal point of the mosquito bite. And so these things sound abstract, but these things really are real. And we can see them, and we can feel them, if we know where to look. So one last idea. We're trying to get at, OK, where are these memories stored? Where are these facts? Where is what you know now about neurobiology stored? And it's helpful to introduce the idea of neural networks. There's 100 billion neurons in the brain. These are not communicating one to one. These are communicating with tens of thousands of other types of neurons. And if you simplify this down to just the basic idea of a neural network, such that you have your bottom, first layer cells. And these first layer cells respond to, respectively, left to right, Monet, Cezanne, Degas. They just respond. For some reason, they have been tuned. They have undergone LTP. That is what they respond to in this very one-to-one Hubel and Wiesel kind of way. But what we notice is that there's this elaborate property you get when you start to combine neurons with many, many other types of neurons, which is that you get a network. And you get a network without one-to-one correspondence. So if you look at the top row, and you get neurons A through E, A still responds in this one-to-one way, with just Monet. E, again, you got just Degas. So those are not really informative in the way in which we want to understand the emergent property, and what's important about neural networks. What we get out of neural networks is emphasized when we focus on C. Neuron C doesn't know-- if it gets activated, what can you tell? You don't know which input it came from. You don't know whether it came from the first-layer neuron 1, first-layer neuron 2, or 3. You don't know whether or not it was a Monet, Cezanne, or Degas. All you know is that it's one of those three. And what you get now is this idea that you can have concepts, and you can have categories. And you can have a category of impressionism. That doesn't necessarily give you information about individual types or names, or which neuron it came from. But you have a network of neurons with different concepts in it. And amidst this network, you can now understand how it is that environment and context can impinge on the storage and retrieval of facts. So the idea that emotionally salient memories are longer lived in your brain, in your synapses, in your plasticity, than other ones, well, how is that true if they're not contextually related, if the mechanism is the same everywhere? But what you begin to see is that if you combine context in this version of neural networks, you start to get the neural representation of context, the neural representation of environment. And this makes sense if you think about how we try to remember things. If you try to remember something, and you know it's an impressionist painter, or you know it's within a category, but you're not quite there, you kind of take a tour of categorical ways of thinking and categorical learning and categorical objects in the world to try to get at how that one fact, that one bit of information that you're trying to remember. So it's not that individual memories are stored in neurons. It's not that they're stored in the generation of synapses. It's not that they are stored in entirely just the plasticity of single synapses. It seems that we can get at and explain a lot of these types of memory by understanding that memory is one aspect of the formation of these neural networks, and that if we have 100 billion neurons, we can imagine elaborate and complex ways of designing these things. So here we go, one more time. Many very different things happen when we remember, everything down from the synaptic plasticity all the way up to this impressionism, categorical way of thinking and remembering about things. And what is again interesting here is that you can imagine-- what we've learned about polymorphisms, genetic individuality and variation, that certain people can have different stress responses. The person next to you can have a different response to stress than the other person. One person will be more afraid of public speaking than the other person. One person will respond a certain way based on prenatal, postnatal environment, all these different things, all these different variations, these polymorphisms that lead to individual and varied behavior. And now we can understand that a polymorphism in how much presynaptic glutamate gets released-- remember, glutamate, excitatory-- a polymorphism in how strongly your postsynaptic receptor responds, a polymorphism in the ways in which your neural networks are constructed, these types of individual things, which each are their own variable in your brain's construct of memory, can lead to different and individual ways in which we remember. Some people are just better at remembering than others. And what we're trying to get at is from the spectrum of HM, who can't remember anything, to Stephen Wiltshire, who can remember this, and where the genetics and the environment impact our individual memory. And I think that's it. So we'll take a five minute break. [APPLAUSE] I'm going to talk to you guys about the autonomic nervous system. So basically, autonomic sounds like automatic. This is anything that's going to happen automatically in your body, not quite the hippocampus, horse, like hippodrome-- like automatic, autonomic. So basically, your heart beating, digesting, goosebumps, orgasm, things that you don't have control over-- [LAUGHTER] Good stuff, right? This is going to be your autonomic nervous system. So first, the nervous system, remember, is split up into the central and the peripheral. So our central nervous system is our brain and our spinal cord. And our peripheral nervous system is everything else on the periphery. And then within that, the peripheral nervous system can be split up into the somatic nervous system and the autonomic. So we're going over the autonomic. Remember that. But just to tell you about the somatic, that's basically the voluntary nervous system. So if you want to pick up a pen off the ground, your brain says, OK, I want to pick up a pen. Send the message to my muscle. Muscle's going to pick up the pan. It's also your sensory info, so when you touch something or smell something, information from the periphery going to your central nervous system. And autonomic nervous system, what we're going to talk about today, can be split up into the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous systems. We'll go over all those in detail. But for right now, one last comparison of the voluntary and autonomic. So the voluntary nervous system, remember, voluntary, moves muscles. Autonomic, it's involuntary, moving organs, your heart, your digestive system, your lungs. The voluntary nervous system's actually myelinated. So what that means is there's a myelin sheath covering the axon, as you can see there. And the action potential actually can speed up and go down the axon faster. And the autonomic nervous system's actually unmyelinated. These are just fun facts. So it goes a little bit slower. The good stuff, autonomic nervous system-- so we have sympathetic and parasympathetic. And sympathetic is that nervous system where you hear fight or flight. So anything exciting, arousal, alertness, emergency, like if you have a hippo chasing after you or something, definitely sympathetic nervous system. If you like somebody and are talking to them first time, sympathetic nervous system activation, you're excited. Parasympathetic is more of the calm, vegetative function. So after you have a huge meal, or when you want to take a nap, anything like that, growth, repair, total relaxation state. And as you can see, they kind of sound like they have opposing functions, because they do. And they tend to work in opposition, so it's kind of like putting your foot on the gas and the brake at the same time. You can't really do that, because they're opposing. When the parasympathetic system is on, your sympathetic nervous system is usually off, and vice versa. So they work together to keep our body going automatically. OK, sympathetic nervous system-- so remember, this is like that huge animal, whatever your favorite one is, chasing after you. What do you do? Well, your heart speeds up. It's going to beat faster. You're going to breathe more. You're going to vasoconstrict. So what that means is you're sending the blood-- you're basically constricting your blood vessels and sending blood more to your lungs and to your muscles, so you can run away. You're going to inhibit digestion. When you're running away from a hippo, you don't care about digesting the sandwich you just had. You're going to sweat. Your muscles will tense, anything you would think of when you're just totally freaked out. And the parasympathetic nervous system-- I really like these pictures. I found the dog, and I got super excited. And then I found him, and I wanted to name him, but I haven't thought of it yet. But basically, they're resting and digesting. They're just taking it easy, like growth, repair. Basically anything you would do when you're not stressed, you have time to do now. Your immune system can function well. You can spend time digesting and urinating. Sympathetic nervous system-- So we're going to look into the neurotransmitters involved in both the symptoms now. So neurons-- what's being communicated? And I know that Pat told you glutamate's the best. But I'm going to fight that and tell you that norepinephrine is one of the good ones, too. So basically, you release norepinephrine in the target organs when you're dealing with the sympathetic nervous system. So the hippo coming at you, what you do is you're going to release norepinephrine, NE, onto the target organs. And you can see the organs on the right. It affects all of those. So it's going to your heart. It's going to your lungs. It's going to your kidney, your bladder. And it's telling it-- when it receives norepinephrine, those organs know, OK, my sympathetic nervous system is activated. I'm going to fight or flight. I'm going to run away right now. Or I'm going to start-- my heart's going to beat faster. And the one exception is the sympathetic nervous system actually releases epinephrine in the adrenal. And this is just a cool exception. Epinephrine, remember it's one step away from norepinephrine in the biosynthetic pathway. So you can make epinephrine from norepinephrine, so they're not really that different. And epinephrine's also called adrenaline-- adrenal, adrenaline, see the resemblance. And this is just another diagram, again, showing you norepinephrine released on the target organ. So you think of sympathetic, you think of norepinephrine. And you can see how it will go and accelerate the heart beat, stuff like that. And just more in detail, if you've taken bio core-- I don't know about [INAUDIBLE] bio core, but definitely bio core-- you know that it's not that simple. You don't need to worry about this. But there's actually an intermediate step, where the spinal cord projections actually first go to this ganglion, which then goes to the target organ and releases NE there. But don't worry about that. Just know norepinephrine, sympathetic. Parasympathetic nervous system-- so we have another cool neurotransmitter besides glutamate and NE, which is acetylcholine, or ACh. And the parasympathetic, you see it goes to all the same organs. But now, when it releases ACh, those organs know parasympathetic, rest and digest. I have time to finish my meal and do everything that I can do when I want to relax. And again, there is an intermediate step, where you release acetylcholine first in the target organ. And then a second neuron goes, releases acetylcholine again. This [INAUDIBLE] Ach, parasympathetic. And if you want more details about it, too, this slide is totally extra details. But you can see the projections from the spinal cord actually lead from different places in the parasympathetic and the sympathetic nervous system. And you can just see at the end, acetylcholine and norepinephrine being released. So this is a really important slide. That's why I put stars on it. [LAUGHTER] Even Sapolsky, when he saw my PowerPoint, he was like, spend a lot of time on that slide. So I'm going to. So we're going to look at exactly what happens when your parasympathetic or sympathetic nervous systems are activated, and compare them in different organs. So the easiest one to start with is your cardiovascular system, so your heart. You're running away, you're scared, or you're meeting someone new for the first time that you really like. And your sympathetic nervous system turns on. Your heart's going to beat faster. Remember that. So your heart actually has a myogenic rhythm, which means it actually has a muscle that is controlling its beating. But what the brain does, and the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system does, is it can change how fast the heart beats. So your heart's beating faster. Your blood pressure will increase when your sympathetic nervous system is on. You're going to vasoconstrict, remember, send the blood to your muscles, so you can run away and all that good stuff. And parasympathetic-- opposite. Slower heart beat, vasodilation of the vessels. Blood's now going to the GI tract for digestion, and everything like that. Another fun example is the GI tract itself, so your gut, your stomach, your small intestine. So basically, parasympathetic activity, when you're resting, you have time to digest. So what you do is you stimulate the secretion of the acids and enzymes needed for digestion. You move your small intestine with a contraction called peristalsis. And basically, you can go to the bathroom, and everything that you would do while you're relaxing. Sorry. So in the heart and the GI tract, you can pretty much see that they're working in opposition. So when the heart beats up with sympathetic, it slows down with parasympathetic. GI, the opposite case, parasympathetic turns it on, speeds up digestion. Parasympathetic turns it off. I'm sorry. This is the important slide. So one place where they actually do work together instead of actually opposing each other is in the male reproductive system. And they work together for you to erect and ejaculate. So what happens is in order to have an erection, you have to be stress free. You can't be worrying about your test. So which one do you think is in charge of erection, parasympathetic or sympathetic? Parasympathetic. Perfect. So parasympathetic activation, you get an erection. Now let's say you have an erection. And now you're with somebody, maybe. I don't know what you're doing. [LAUGHTER] Whatever's happening-- sorry. All of a sudden, you feel your heart beating faster. You start sweating a little bit. Your sympathetic nervous system is turned on a little bit now. So now we have parasympathetic, we still have our erection. But we also have some sympathetic activity, and then more and more sympathetic activity. And all of a sudden, sympathetic activity completely takes over. And what happens? You ejaculate, right? So parasympathetic-- erection, sympathetic-- ejaculation. And it's actually a cool fact about erectile dysfunction that about 60% of the cases are due to stress and not actually organic basis in your body. So if you're stressed out all of the time, your parasympathetic activity won't turn on, so you can't have an erection. And also, we can explain premature ejaculation, if you want to to your friends tonight. You can just be like, well, let's think about it. So I have an erection, but I'm going to ejaculate too soon. So parasympathetic transition to sympathetic transition, or the parasympathetic transition to sympathetic, happens too quickly, your premature ejaculation. [LAUGHTER] And then health, so immune system. [LAUGHTER] When your parasympathetic system is on, you can take care of your immune system. You have the time to make the white blood cells. But when you're chasing away from a predator or an elephant, you really don't care about making new white blood cells. And this could also explain why it's easier to get sick when you're stressed out. Your sympathetic is too much caring about your stressful situation than taking care of your immune system. I don't know-- oh my computer goes on sleep. I think that's it. So again, we see there's a balance between the two branches. So sympathetic, you're running away from a snake. When that's on, parasympathetic's off, and vice versa. And there's actually a really cute video that I found. And you have to click it twice. So the sympathetic nervous system, this video will tell you everything that I just told you. It increases heart rate, makes your pupils dilate so you can see further, run away from the predator. You don't have time to digest. You don't care about nasal secretions right now. [LAUGHTER] You're not going to produce saliva. Who cares about eating? Inhibits the liver, kidneys, and gall bladder, and stimulates sweating. We're going to sweat when we're running away, getting scared. Causes piloerection, so when your hair stands when you're nervous. Makes the lungs dilate, you can breathe faster. Increases muscle strength so you can run away, and is important for orgasm. [LAUGHTER] Sorry. Parasympathetic, opposite, so it makes your heart rate go down. Pupils are going to contract. You're going to digest. You're going to like the nasal secretions now. [LAUGHTER] You're going to stimulate the liver, the bladder, and the kidneys. You constrict your lungs. You're going to pay more attention to your digestion. And it's important for sexual arousal, remember erections. You can play it again later. So an important point to make is when we think about sympathetic nervous system, we're thinking about arousal, emergency, fight or flight. But that doesn't mean it always goes to the organ and excites it. So in the heart, when norepinephrine goes from the sympathetic nervous system to the heart, it does excite the heart and make it beat faster. But when it goes to the GI tract, it inhibits GI tract activity. So it's not always excitatory, it's not always inhibitory. It depends on the organ. Same with parasympathetic. We think of it as being the slower moving one. But in the GI tract, it does excite it. In the heart, it inhibits. So what does that mean? It means we need two different receptors on our organs that respond to norepinephrine or acetylcholine. So on the heart, for instance, for norepinephrine, you'll have an excitatory norepinephrine receptor, because it will get excited and make the heart beat faster. But in the GI tract, you'll have an inhibitory norepinephrine receptor that will respond to the sympathetic nervous system and slow it down. And then for the parasympathetic, you'd have an excitatory ACh receptor on your GI tract to speed it up, to digest more food. And you'll have an inhibitory ACh receptor on the heart to slow it down. So just see you can't always have the same receptor on the same organ, or else it wouldn't respond right. And this is just showing you, again. So the heart there, you have your inhibitory ACh receptor, which tells your heart to slow down. Excitatory norepinephrine, heart speeds up. And in the GI tract, if ACh is coming your way, it will attach to the excitatory receptor, and it'll be like, digest. And you have the inhibitory norepinephrine receptor there, too. So if sympathetic activity is being stimulated, norepinephrine will land there and will slow down digestion. So if you've taken bio core, if you want to know more-- there's actually names for all of these forms of receptors that I put there, just in case you're extra interested. But on the heart, I think the coolest fact about it is the beta blocker. So the form of the receptor on the heart that responds to the sympathetic nervous system is actually called a beta receptor. And what beta blockers do, they block the receptor, the beta receptor. So this is why beta blockers are used for slowing down heart rate, reducing hypertension. It's basically blocking the effects of the sympathetic nervous system. And Pat actually just told me, which is great, that the one drug that's banned from the Olympics are actually beta blockers, because if you think about it, a huge advantage would be to be less stressed. So if they're blocking the receptor on your heart that responds to stress and the sympathetic nervous system, you can see how it could allow you to relax more. So it's a fun fact. So now we're going to talk about the regulation of the autonomic nervous system, so what's happening in the brain that's resulting in norepinephrine or acetylcholine being released. And the center of regulation is now the hypothalamus. So we just talked about the hippocampus, so this is a different area of the brain, the hypothalamus. It's going to be very important on Monday as well, when Tom and Will talk about the endocrine system, because the hypothalamus directly affects the pituitary gland, which is center of your endocrine system. So basically, the hypothalamus here contains the cell bodies, or is just one synapse away from all the cell bodies that project onto the target organs, from the spinal cord to the target organs. So basically, the hypothalamus will tell the spinal cord what to project onto the organ. So an example of this would be in your heart. And this is actually called the baroreflex. And this is just an example of how your hypothalamus is going to help your body maintain status quo, so make sure that your blood pressure's never too high, your heart's beating at a normal speed. So let's say you're hemorrhaging, because I don't know, a hyena just attacked you. That's weird. So anyways, you're hemorrhaging. And you're losing a lot of blood, so your blood pressure's going to go way down. And you have these receptors in your blood vessels that are called baroreceptors. And they'll say, OK, blood pressure's way too low. What do I do? They're going to send that info to the hypothalamus-- remember the hypothalamus, center of regulation. And the hypothalamus will be in charge of sending that information along to the spinal cord, which will then project onto the heart and tell it to beat faster. Sympathetic nervous system will be activated. Beat faster, increase my blood pressure, so that you'll make up for the loss of blood that you just had. And the opposite would happen if your blood pressure is getting too high or something. Maybe the info will be sent to your brain. And then you can decrease blood pressure through the parasympathetic nervous system. So reptiles, everybody kind of has that hypothalamus control of the-- yeah. Sorry, I had a question about the [INAUDIBLE]. This is kind of a [INAUDIBLE] question, but is it actually stimulating the sympathetic nervous system? Or is it normally stimulating the parasympathetic and then stops the stimulation? Oh, because remember, one's on or off. So like which one's normally on? Do you guys know? Anybody know? There's just a baseline balance [INAUDIBLE]. Yeah. So what about mammals? Mammals have emotions. And we have an emotional regulation are in our brain that's called the limbic system. And we're going to learn a whole lecture just about the limbic system in general. But basically, it has everything do with emotions, behaviors, memories, all mammalian type things. So now we see this realm where not just losing all your blood can activate or stimulate the nervous system and cause a parasympathetic or sympathetic response. But now just seeing someone you hate can cause a sympathetic response that's very similar to losing a lot of blood. And this is pretty amazing. Wildebeests, for instance, if they see their enemies, the info will be sent from just the smell of their enemies to the limbic system, project onto the hypothalamus, spinal cord, to the sympathetic nervous system, be like, I don't like you response. The sympathetic nervous system wanting to either fight or flight. And then in the realm of primates, we also have our cortex. And what the cortex does is it makes thoughts and memories really important. So now, instead of just having-- losing a lot of blood, we're changing how our body functions. And now, not even having to sense-- we don't need to have a sense. We can just think about a thought. And that can go ahead and change the way that every organ in our body functions, which is pretty amazing. So when you're thinking about a test, for instance, you're going to activate your cortex. And this will activate your limbic system, and then your hypothalamus. So that's known, actually, as the triune system of the brain. You have the cortex in primates, mostly. Then you have the limbic system, mostly mammalian. And then you have the hypothalamus. So it's going to go to each one of these layers of the brain. And just thinking about a test can cause a sympathetic response, where you start sweating, getting nervous, stressed out. And it's pretty amazing that if you lost a lot of blood in a reptile, we can stimulate the same response just thinking about something, or thinking about someone on the other side of the world dying. It's just amazing what primates can do. And an interesting example of this-- and I think we're having a lecture on depression, so I don't want to give it all away yet. But if you think about it, the symptoms of depression, loss of pleasure, pain pathways on, don't want to have sex, aren't in the mood to eat, you're exhausted all the time, a lot of these symptoms are the same symptoms you would see if your sympathetic nervous system was overly activated. And we can see how the cortex having bad thoughts can go and activate that system in the same way, links to depression. And the last thing I wanted to talk about in terms of the autonomic nervous system was the plasticity of it. So we just learned the plasticity in neurons, in the synapses. So that's when it can change over time. And the autonomic nervous system can actually change over time, in terms of how receptive, or when it turns on and off. And a molecular example of this is if you're a very stressful person, and you're stressing all the time. Well, then you need a lot of norepinephrine. What do you do if you're stressing all the time? You increase the synthesis of the enzyme called tyrosine hydroxylase, I believe. Yeah, it's up there. And basically, this is the rate limiting step in making norepinephrine. So if you increase more of the enzyme, you increase more of the norepinephrine, you can sustain the stress response. Another example, cellular example, is that we have projections from the sympathetic nervous system to the skin, eyes, nose, everything that's going on out there. And let's say we see something scary. We can make those receptors more sensitive to that scary thing. So we can say, hey, it actually smells that enemy. We can make it seem scarier. The sympathetic nervous system can turn on faster, so sensitization. There's also the opposite end of the spectrum, where you habituate to things that are going on outside. So scary stimuli-- if you see a spider in your room the first time, you're probably going to freak out when you're younger. Lots of sympathetic activity, running away, fight or flight. You decide to fly, because I don't like spiders. But basically, after a while, second time you see a spider, you're just like, oh, this is still scary. Maybe I'll run away. The third time, maybe you decide, whatever, I'm just going to leave it there at this point. And you're habituated to it. So you've made the thresholds of your sensory receptors, they don't care as much. They don't respond as much. And a last example, when we're talking about cognitive thought and the cortex and what that can do to change our autonomic nervous system, is an example of biofeedback and blood pressure. So basically, if you have high blood pressure, you can go into the doctor's office. And you have two options. You can take medicine, or you could try biofeedback. And what they do is they tell you to think of a pleasant thought. So think of your favorite vacation, or think of your favorite person, or just think of the beach in general. And what you'll see is that your blood pressure will actually decrease with a certain thought. And then the doctor will tell you to think about that thought again. Your blood pressure will decrease. And thinking more and more about that thought, helping your blood pressure decrease, what you do is you potentiate, remember, you make stronger the connection where a cortical thought can go ahead and activate more parasympathetic tone, have less sympathetic tone. So we're potentiating that pathway by which a thought can cause our blood pressure to decrease, which is pretty cool. So the take home points, if you want to just know what to remember from this. Know the broad difference between autonomic, automatic, and the voluntary nervous system, what we talked about at the beginning. Understand the neurotransmitters involved in each, and why you need two types of receptors, the inhibitory and excitatory. Know one or two examples of what the parasympathetic-- that's what PNS, by the way, means-- and sympathetic nervous system do to an organ. So remember the heart, the digestive tract, the male reproductive system. And then know a broad overview of how the brain regulates the autonomic nervous system, so hypothalamus, cortex, and we have the limbic system. On Monday, we're going over endocrinology. So have a good-- For more, please visit us at Stanford.edu.
Info
Channel: Stanford
Views: 336,576
Rating: 4.8681445 out of 5
Keywords: memory, plasticity, human biology, neurobiology, interdisciplinary, science, autonomic nervous system, context, brain, synapse, neuron, response, cell, activation, neurotransmitter, hippocampus, neurogenesis, ltp, emotion, saliency, individual variation
Id: uqU9lmFztOU
Channel Id: undefined
Length: 73min 2sec (4382 seconds)
Published: Tue Feb 01 2011
Related Videos
Note
Please note that this website is currently a work in progress! Lots of interesting data and statistics to come.