15. Human Sexual Behavior I

Video Statistics and Information

Video
Captions Word Cloud
Reddit Comments

I can't recommend enough people take the full course. It's amazing.

👍︎︎ 10 👤︎︎ u/Erinaceous 📅︎︎ Dec 09 2013 🗫︎ replies

He has many more amazing lectures. My favorite one is about depression, highly recommended

👍︎︎ 11 👤︎︎ u/Jose_Monteverde 📅︎︎ Dec 09 2013 🗫︎ replies

"May 5, 2010) Robert Sapolsky explores behavioral patterns of human reproduction. He focuses on proximal and distal motivations, orgasm and fertility facilitation, non-reproductive sex, hormonal and cerebral sexual functions, and the differences and similarities between humans and animals in various physiological realms"

👍︎︎ 4 👤︎︎ u/Telmid 📅︎︎ Dec 09 2013 🗫︎ replies

This guy (Robert Sapolsky) is wicked smart and completely accessible--totally would recommend this and his other stuff on iTunes U.

👍︎︎ 3 👤︎︎ u/DescriptiveEthics 📅︎︎ Dec 09 2013 🗫︎ replies

He has some Teaching Company lectures too, IIRC, but I forget what they cover.

👍︎︎ 2 👤︎︎ u/[deleted] 📅︎︎ Dec 09 2013 🗫︎ replies

I watched all of these available on youtube. He's a fabulous lecturer.

👍︎︎ 2 👤︎︎ u/[deleted] 📅︎︎ Dec 09 2013 🗫︎ replies
Captions
[MUSIC PLAYING] Stanford University. Is this on? Yes. Well, congratulations. Everybody has survived the midterm including the TAs so far, who recently have been let out of their entombment with thousands of pages of exam pages. So those are rolling along, and everybody is still awake so that's good. OK. So we have now officially entered the second half of the course. And organizational things-- readings will now be read hopefully. OK. So that's not useful. Books. Books. Now is a good time to actually go and start reading those books. And again, if you are not up to all of both of them, some recommend the chapters have come along. Pay attention to those. And again, a subset of you-- may not know it yet, but about 25% of you will have your life transformed by that chaos book, whereas 25% will resent the purchase price and my forcing you to do this. Also, another major, major transition here, which is, as far as I can tell so far, I have run out of steam turning the extended notes into actual expository writing, so they're just going to take the form of really poorly organized outlines. So that happens. OK. What else? What we now transition to is going to be our strategy for the rest of our course, which is to look at various subjects. And coming down the pike after the lectures today and Friday on sexual behavior will be aggression, competition, cooperation, empathy, potentially language use, schizophrenia. Somewhere in there, there is going to be a week or so on that chaos stuff. But for all of these subjects, we are now going to follow the general strategy. We will start off looking at what the behavior is. And what we're going to try to do there, in addition to looking at in lots of different species, is to be as objective as any good old ethologist in considering the fixed action patterns of the particular behavior. That established, with the promise that, in lots of cases, what we think comprise some of these behaviors turn out not to be the case. That established, what we will start is our inexorable march to the left, the timeline that constitutes everything we learn now, stepping back and saying, OK. One millisecond before that behavior occurred, what was going on in the brain? What parts of the brain? What neurotransmitters? All of that. Stepping back before that, a second before, a minute before, whatever, what was it in the environment that triggered the brain to produce that behavior? What was the acute releasing stimulus? Stepping back, what do hormone levels that hour, that day, some such time span of that, what acute hormonal exposure had to do with sensitizing you to the environmental stimulus which released the nervous system into generating the behavior. Marching all the way back, throw in culture some place or other. Perinatal effects, early developmental hormonal stuff, eventually considering what do the genetics of the individual, of the population, of the species-- evolution kicking in there some place or other-- what do all of these things and something having to do with ecology thrown in there just for good measure. Working our way back in each of these cases, understanding what was the biology of one second before, one minute, one hour, one million years before that gave rise to it. Back to our two major themes from day one, we are now about to be unbound, unfettered, by our buckets, our categorical buckets, and instead explore their interactions. The other being that notion from the very first class, which is that any given point, if we're talking about chronic hormonal effects on this behavior, what we're talking about is the way those hormone patterns were shaped during this period, the way genes contributed to the enzymes that make the hormones, the receptors. The second you're talking about genetics, all of this is becoming apparent. At every one of these points, whatever point we are talking about is going to be the end product of everything to the left and just as temporary sort of footing before going to the things on the right. So this will be our strategy forever after now. So we start off doing this with looking at sexual behavior, the neurobiology, the endocrinology, the early experiences, et cetera, et cetera. So to inaugurate the second half of the course and the fact that it's starting off with lectures on sexual behavior, it has to start off with the stupid obligatory sex joke. OK. So the Martians come to Earth, and they turn out to be great guys. They are really terrific. They get along wonderfully with Earthlings. All of them like each other a lot. They're all becoming great friends. They pass their hours learning about each other's planets. What's the weather like there? What are sports like here? What are recipes? Everybody's getting along terrifically. And eventually, the Earthlings and the Martians are getting along well enough that they get around to asking the question that everybody is really interested in, which is, well, how do you folks go about reproducing? So the decision was made. The Martians are going to go first. So they clear out a big space, and a whole bunch of Martians come in there and they stand on top of each other heads, and their noses flash different colors, and steam comes out of various orifices, and there's clanking noises and whatever. And out pops a new little Martian. And the Earthlings say, wow, that was great. Love the concept. And I got great video of that. And that's terrific and all of that. OK So that's worked out. And now it's the humans turn. So a willing couple is found, and some space is cleared out. And the Martians sit down there with their video cameras as well. And clicking away. And this couple goes at it. And they finish the whole thing in a sweaty mess at the end. And the Martian say, that's wonderful. That's so interesting. You Earthlings are just endless. And the fascinating things you do, but we have one question though, which is, so where's the new human? And they said, oh, that takes nine months. They say, well, why were they in such a rush at the end? So our first question here is, why were they in such a rush at the end? OK. Three possible answers. Choice number one-- vote for it-- why were they in such a rush at the end? Number one, because they were acting with this fervent desire to do something for the good of the species. Just seeing if any hands go up. That's a good thing. Option number two, doing that because you want to maximize the number of copies of your genes passed on to the next generation. Option number three, because it feels good. OK. One hand goes up. And I'm not sure what that indicates about everyone, but I will remind you from the survey in the first class there that a far greater percentage of you want to learn about depression than about sexual behavior. So there you have it with the Stanford experience. OK. Because it feels good. And what we deal with here right off the start is this important dichotomy between proximal and distal explanations for behavior. Explanation, a distal explanation, for sexual behavior, parentheses, why were they in such a rush at the end? Passing on copies of your genes, the effects of hormones, and certain reward pathways in the brain, all of that. Proximal mechanism being that it feels good. And starting off right off the bat the thing to make sense of with sexual behavior is it is driven by this amazing little loop here of sensory stimuli, and feedback, and immediate sensations that drive the behavior coming out. And all this stuff down here is for the doctoral thesis somewhere down the line. That's not what the motivation is. Probably more than any other of the behaviors we will look at, the driving forces are very proximal ones. Nobody sits there and figures out how many copies of genes they are passing on and thus are willing to speed up to produce a new human nine months later. It is instead, in species after species, it is proximal motivating mechanisms for generating the behaviors. OK. So beginning to look at the actual behaviors, there is a funny duality to making sense of sexual behavior across different species, a funny sort of contrast. The first one being that, well, all species go about sex-- or all vertebrate species go about sex in a roughly similar way. Yet, you don't want to be too similar to the species next door. There's an interesting sort of dichotomy there. All sorts of vertebrate species are doing things with pelvic thrusting and orgasms and-- hey, stay tuned that's coming-- and ejaculation, and lordotic reflexes, and things of that sort. Highly conserved fixed action patterns across lots and lots of different species. None the less, amid that, you've got this other problem, which is you want to have these fixed action patterns being specific to your species. You do not want to mess up. So there is this strange simultaneity of very, very conserved basic building blocks of the fixed action patterns of sexual behavior. But along with that, a lot of selectivity within species. Now how does that selectivity begin to work? What you get is this very interesting interplay, this intercalation, between the releasing stimuli and the fixed action pattern. What you get is this chaining of behavior. In other words, the fixed action pattern of one of the individuals constitutes the releasing stimulus for the other individual's fixed action pattern, which constitutes the releasing stimulus for this individual's fixed action pattern. This chaining of transitions there, of interplay between these two, which is where you get the species specificity from. OK. So in terms of making sense of that, of course, any of this in terms of looking at the general features of how to think about sexual behavior across species, of course, what you have to have out the wazoo is your basic ethology credo of interviewing an animal in its own language about its sexual behavior, wonderfully summarized in this quote by Martha McClintock, researcher. I think I've used this quote already, which is, in her particular case, studying female rat sexual behavior, which turns out to be this very ornate process involving a lot of running around on the part of the female. Studying female rat sexual behavior in a cage is like trying to study swimming behavior of a dolphin in a bath tub. You need to get it in the natural setting, or else you are going to lose all sorts of insight. In the particular realm of female rat sexual behavior, the standard picture for decades and decades, the centuries where our finest minds have looked at rats having sex, the standard dogma has been that the female role is a very passive receptive one. And it turns out it's a very passive receptive one if she doesn't have enough room to run around and do all sorts of courting stuff on her own, all sorts of proceptive sexual behaviors, which she can't see, if you're studying an animal in a setting where they can't speak in their own language. So a big, big vote for ethological logical principles when it comes to this. All right. So just to get some jargon under our belt right from the start here, in terms of how the professionals talk about sex when they're talking about sex and trying to sound like professionals, here are some of the terms. Old outdated term, Freudian term, that nonetheless has entered the general world of referring to sexual arousal and motivation-- libido. Libido, as we will see that commonplace everyday usage term, is perhaps more technically described as horniness. But it can also be described as one term within a trio of the terms that people in the business really use most frequently-- attractivity, proceptivity, and receptivity. Quick, get to work on poems about those three terms. But what you've got here is attractivity, how attractive an individual is to someone else. Receptivity, how receptive that individual is to the interest of the other individual. Proceptivity, the active behaviors that are carried out in response to being attracted to. And thus, you could say because of the attractiveness of this organism, this other organism began proceptive behaviors which did or did not prove to meet with receptive fixed action patterns and responses. The very words any of us would use to describe what goes on at a party. OK. So we've got that triad there, the terms that are much more in common than terms like libido or arousal or motivation. These are the more common ones. What is another realm, in terms that are much more used, these are much more zoology terms. What's used far more often in clinical medicine is a description, a dichotomy, between motivation and performance. And that is never used more frequently than in the realm of making sense of sexual motivation in men as dissociable from erectile function versus dysfunction, motivation being very, very different from performance. So that's another realm of distinctions. Other realms as well-- desire, orgasms, arousal, all sorts of commonplace terms. The performance versus motivation dichotomy and the proceptivity, receptivity, [INAUDIBLE] are the major terms that are used. Next issue, in terms of getting to this, how do people find out information about sexual behavior? One option is to sit there with night viewing goggles. And that's very useful for nocturnal species. But how do people find out about human sexual behavior? All sorts of ways over the years, starting with anonymous questionnaires. But a really clever technique was worked out in the 1980s. A biological mathematician named Joel Cohen getting at how to get people to tell you about very embarrassing things about their sexual lives. And this was prompted in the 80s when AIDS first swept in, and it was wildly, wildly taboo at the time in virtually every corner of this country to admit to having a less than standard, white bread sexual orientation. Take a look at the extended notes to see the trick that Joel Cohen came up with, a very clever device in order to figure out what percentage of people are doing what sexually without asking anybody to give an answer that they would find, perhaps, to be embarrassing or grounds for all sorts of persecution. OK. So beginning to look at aspects of behavior and other features of the rightmost end of all of this. We start off with the most central puzzle in making sense of any of this stuff, which is, so what's up with female orgasms? And we've got right off the bat the simple problem of making sense of this biological phenomenon and one where fertility is not dependent on it. One does not need to have orgasms to become pregnant, to give birth, to pass on copies of one's genes. So what's the deal with orgasms? First off, a question we will wind up asking with a whole lot of the behavior coming down the line is, are we the only species? And all sorts of careful studies have shown that we are not the only species. Other apes, other primate species, as well monkeys and apes, show orgasm among the females. And that is detectable by all sorts of physiology we'll hear about in a while. We are not the only species. One of the really bizarre, pathetic things about trying to do research in this area was one of the first papers that ever demonstrated something which physiologically was identical to female orgasm in rhesus monkey females, which wound up being a paper in this journal Science. Down there in the footnotes, the authors had to indicate this did not make use of any federal grant money to carry out the study. Just to give you a sense of where some of the stuff sits. OK. So what's up with female orgasm? It is not necessary for conception. It is not necessary to increase the number of copies of your genes in future generations. Despite that, there is some evidence that it facilitates fertilization. And the technical term that's always been given for that is, bizarrely enough, facilitation. The notion is something about the vaginal fluid, something about the biochemistry of, increases sperm motility. Sperm swim faster and harder and jump upstream back to spawn or whatever it is the sperm are doing with more avidity, with more energetic displays, in an environment of more vaginal secretion. And orgasm greatly increases that. So the argument there being that orgasm facilitates fertilization through the sperm facilitation process. Evidence for that has always been a little bit indirect. It is not airtight that that happens. Another argument for why this increases fertility. And this is sort of an interesting one. And the notion here is that what an orgasm does is, among other things, exhausts you enormously, causing you to be far more likely to be horizontal than vertical shortly thereafter, and thus, facilitating fertilization because the sperm don't have to swim straight up against gravity. I kid you not. This is one of the leading models out there. Then there's the orgasm facilitates female conception out of reinforcement theory, which is back to the it feels good and thus you are more likely to do it again and increasing the likelihood of passing on copies of your genes. All of this is wonderful. All of these possible mechanisms where, even though female orgasm is not necessary for conception, it nonetheless increases the likelihood of. That's great. However, what most of the studies have shown, though, is there is no relationship between the fertility of a woman and her propensity towards orgasm. It does not seem to play out in so far as any of this facilitation or horizontal swim enhancement techniques or whatever actually occurs-- these are not big enough of effects to actually make a difference in terms of reproductive success. So what else? What else is known about it? There is a certain degree of heritability, of propensity towards orgasm in females. And this is shown with all our standard behavior genetics techniques in terms of comparing dizygotic versus monozygotic twins. We know what to do in terms of not overvaluing findings like those. Nonetheless, they are there. So if a basic puzzle is, why do females have orgasms if it's not necessary for conception and, in fact, the evidence is not great that it even facilitates it, why on top of all of that, why such things as clitoral orgasms, which the studies generally show are more easily brought about than are vaginal ones, what's up with that? Even more the same question. Now somewhere in there is a lurking the heart breaking possibility in making sense of why female orgasm exists the dreadful possibility that what we're dealing with here is a spandrel. And that it is a spandrel. It is simply baggage brought along that those guys have to go through this orgasm physiology to do any stuff with sperm and pass on copies of genes and all of that. And it's simply baggage that the same physiology occurs in females. That orgasm is simply a spandrel in women. And the counter scenario that's always given is, this is exactly equivalent to the notion that nipples are spandrels in men, in that women, female mammals, need to go about all this lactation business. And that's part of the whole package deal. And just as it would be way too much work to evolve females without orgasms, it would be way too much to get rid of those useless nipples on men. OK. Let's have a quick survey. Yes? So could a lot of those questions also be applied to why do male have orgasms? Because ejaculation can occur without orgasm. OK. So why do males have orgasms? Ejaculation can occur-- it is far more voluminous in the face of an orgasm. So that's easily framed in terms of an adaptive mechanism. OK. Quick survey here. How many people-- OK, how many guys who have those useless nipples, how many of you go for the nipple as spandrel in guys theory? Whoa. Is that slow in the hands? OK. Should I raise my hand? Should I not raise my hand? OK. Women in the room, how many go for the female orgasm is merely spandrel theory? If I recall, there was one other question somewhere a few weeks back that only got one person fessing up to it, and it came somewhere around there also. So I don't know if that's the lighting or if people tend to sit in the same places. OK. So not a whole lot of enthusiasm for the spandrel concepts here. Nonetheless, that needs to be seriously entertained. OK. So now looking at other features of the fixed action patterns, and amid all these different species doing lordotic stuff and orgasms and ejaculation and all of that, what are some of the realms of sexual behavior that are relatively unique to humans? First off, one that used to be thought to be absolutely unique was non-reproductive sex. This is a world of difference than those species where the female ovulates for like 2 and 1/2 hours every year, and everybody mates at that point. Or species where somebody comes into heat, a female is in estrus. Humans have non-reproductive sex. And that was viewed as absolutely unique. What it is now clear is it is not completely unique. There are lots of other species that do, probably most famously bonobo chimps and various cetaceans like dolphins. Nonetheless, it is certainly a specialty among humans. What else? Foreplay, that used to be in the category of human specialties. And it is clear by now that bonobo chimps, for example, have vastly more patience with foreplay than average humans do. We are not the only species with that either. Huge, huge controversy. How unique is homosexuality to human behavior? Human sexual behavior? And what's clear increasingly is we're not the only species with that either. The original view, when people would view homosexual behavior, male-male, female-female, in other species, it would be animals in captivity. And it would be the, why is there so much homosexuality in prisons argument-- lack of alternatives. This was not normal, natural behavior. What is clear from ethological field studies is we are by no means the only species to have homosexual behavior. What else? One of the things that we do seem to be fairly unique about is having egalitarian sex, which is to say that there are no human cultures where as part of the central tenets of that culture people are restricted, only a subset of individuals are allowed to reproduce. And this is a world of difference from various species. For example, New World monkeys, marmosets, where it is only one male and one female in a group that does the reproducing. Instead, humans in every culture ever seen have egalitarian sex. What else? What else is highly human? People used to think exclusively so this endless quest for variety. And again, just take a look at these bonobo chimps, and you'll see how small minded we are when it comes to this. But something else that is indeed unique to human sexual behavior is the notion that this is something you do in private. There is no other species where the majority of sexual behavior is conducted intentionally outside the sight of everybody else. That is rather unique to humans. What else about human sexual behavior seems to be specializations? One that is fairly unique, if not entirely so, is the subset of humans who psychopathological confuse sexual behavior with violence. And that seems not to have a whole lot of precedence. So immediately one asks other domains. Masturbation, that is not remotely a human specialty. That has all sorts of other species as well. And that used to get the, well, what else is there to do when you're sitting in the zoo-- for the animals-- what else is there to do? It is not natural. It is a default whatever. But looking out in the real world, and there is plenty of that. And one of the most like implausible suggestions for an adaptive just so story thing is, why do male primates masturbate to the point of ejaculation? They tend to eat the semen afterward. Whoa! Great source of protein, go the adaptationists. Everything has an adaptive basis. This one does not ring terribly true to me. What else? Fantasy. Fantasy in humans, is that unique to humans? Obviously, we haven't a clue. But here's one suggestion to me that this is not actually the case. And this was years ago where I was watching my baboons, and there was this one low ranking kid, this snivelly adolescent kid, where the nearest thing he has ever gotten to a female in his life with some high ranking female in a bad mood beating on him. And he's sitting there minding his own business. And along comes, I think by any baboon male standards, the hottest female in the troop, who has a peek estrus swelling, is no doubt ovulating that day. Comes walking along, followed two feet behind by the huge menacing male, who was in the consortship with her. And our guy just sits there and doesn't even quite look at what's happening. Every now and then, his eyes go up, watching them go past. And as she walks past, he gets an erection and then goes off and masturbates. OK. The charming-- I don't know if this is even the word you can use in this setting-- but the charming notion is that we've just seen evidence for some sort of internal fantasy life going on this guy. OK. You could be a killjoy instead and say, no, no. She was giving off wafts of pheromones, and that was what was responsible for it. Nonetheless this is about as far as we can get at asking this critical question, are we the only species that does this fantasizing stuff? Marriage. Clearly, we've heard about monogamous pair bonding species. In terms of the formal structure of marriage, it is universal. All human cultures have some version of it. Across all human cultures, more than 90% of people wind up in that culture's equivalent of a permanent, stable relationship. And this is the case in polygamous cultures. We've already heard that business. Even though historically, the majority of human cultures have been polygamous, nonetheless, amid them, the vast majority of individuals have been in monogamous relationships. Amid that, nonetheless, what is also clear is amid that highly, highly prevalent pattern of monogamous relationships, there's a lot less monogamy going around than you would think. And this was first sorted out-- people like Alfred Kinsey when first working out that questionnaire approach to people's sexual behavior, what became clear was there is a lot less faith within pair bonding, within humans in this country and has since shown in all sorts of other societies than one would originally assume. There is social monogamy but not necessarily anywhere near as high of rates of sexual monogamy. And what the paternity studies have shown is in most Western European countries, the rate at which children have been fathered by an individual other than the person claiming marriageable credit for doing so ranges between 10% and 40% of children. How's that for a number? OK. What else? What else tends to be a feature of human sexual behavior? What you have is, of course, not only intrinsic in the fact that there's a difference between social and sexual monogamy. You have cheating. That is a human specialty in every culture. What else is absolutely wildly human? This notion of romance. And romance is, by most estimates, a relatively new invention in most cultures, maybe a couple of centuries old. And what is an even newer invention is the notion that romance, passion, et cetera, should persist, should last throughout the entire duration of the lifetime's marriage. That is an utterly novel concept. That is perhaps 30, 40, 50 years old in most Westernized countries. That's a new one as well. What that, of course, ushers in is looking at issues of divorce. Across all cultures, the average duration of marriages are two to four years. And people have made the argument that that is the typical duration of children being dependent on a high degree of parenting, of both parents being around. That is the average interbirth interval, two to four years, in most traditional human cultures. What's the term being described then if that is the "natural" point at which most marriages dissolve and turn into other monogamous relationships? The term that is given is that humans tend toward being serial monogamists, moving from one monogamous relationship to another with, on the average, a lag time roughly corresponding to the interbirth interval. So that's charming. What else? What else do you have? All sorts of other aspects of human sexual variety, but ultimately, when you look at human sexual behavior versus other species, we are so boring. We are so limited when you look at the range of unlikely things going on out there. Species that are regularly hermaphroditic-- and people, in fact, have done studies on how is it that a hermaphroditic animal does not try to have sex with itself? And these are usually worm type things. And that's some version of an incest avoidance. At the same time, there are other species where individuals change sex opportunistically. All sorts of fish species where that happens. Lots of species that are parthenogenetic, where an individual reproduces without the benefit of anybody else's genetic input. Even stranger, there's a bunch of snake species that are parthenogenic, but the females cannot reproduce parthenogenically unless they mate with males. They do not actually get any sperm from the males, and they get no genetic contribution, but something about that is necessary for the parthenogenetic event to occur. OK. So all sorts of bizarrities that make our fixed action patterns look really pretty dull. But nonetheless, these are the backbones of the human fixed action patterns, and some of them wildly unique, some of them far less than people used to think, some of them very, very unprecedented. OK. So what this allows us to do now is make our first big step. What's going on in the brain? What is the neurobiology of sexual behavior producing those fixed action patterns? And what you better bet right off the bat is we are talking about the limbic system. This is all limbic system until we see ways in which it's not just all the limbic system. But it is heavily centered-- no surprise-- in the limbic system. And this was being noted first around the 1930s, 1940s with the first experiments where there were lesion studies done damaging different parts of the limbic system in animals. And what would be noted was animal sexual behavior would change. And this was eventually termed a profile, termed after the two scientists who pioneered this stuff called the Kluvre-Bucy Syndrome, which is when you damage some of these strange mysterious rhinencephalonic structures in there, you change the sexual behavior. You change, for example, in monkeys, whether they are attempting to mate with another monkey as opposed to an inanimate object. They change aspects of the fixed action patterns of the behavior and such. And out of it, this was one of the main driving forces on people saying, nose-brain, well, that's great. But actually, what we're looking at is a part of the brain that has lots to do with emotion and emotionally related behaviors. That was one of the driving forces on the limbic system being pulled together as a concept. OK. So what areas within the limbic system are relevant? First pass, there are different hot spots in there depending on gender. Among females, probably the most important area is a subsection of the hypothalamus called the ventral medial hypothalamus involved in female sexual behavior. What's the evidence for that? Just go back to last Friday's lecture-- lesion studies, stimulation studies, recording studies. Destroy the VMH, you do not get sexual behavior anymore from a female. Stimulate it, and you will get the same behaviors that you would normally only see in an ovulating female rat for example. All the sorts of tools we heard about. Reinforcing this even more is this is the hot spot in the hypothalamus for receptors for estrogen and progesterone. So that makes lots of sense. Meanwhile, another region of the brain that is typically involved in sexual behavior in females, a region in the midbrain. The midbrain, which seems to have something to do with some of the hormonal aspects of sexual behavior that are specific to females. Finally, back to that lordosis reflex, you got to have a spinal cord to pull off the full array of typical mammalian female sexual behavior. So spinal pathways, which do not exist in males, lordotic reflexes, the back arching reflex, is exclusively a female one. Meanwhile, over on the other side of things, there are regions in the brain that tend to be more specialized for sexual behavior in males than in females-- a different part of the hypothalamus called the medial preoptic area. And the exact same sort of evidence is for the ventral medial hypothalamus in females-- lesion studies, stimulation, recording studies, all that sort of thing, and-- you guessed it-- whopping great amounts of testosterone receptors, androgen receptors, within the medial preoptic area. Very interestingly, something we will hear more about next week or so, is another region of the brain is involved in male sexual behavior, which is the amygdala. Mhm. That's kind of interesting. The amygdala. We've already heard about amygdala fear, anxiety, and all of that. But the amygdala also plays a very major role in aggression. And there's a little bit, a small domain, of amygdaloid function in males that's involved in sexual behavior, involved in sexual motivation. Medial preoptic area is much more about sexual performance in males. Amygdala is much more about sexual motivation. And all sorts of people have speculated fairly reasonably, I think, that this may have something to do with the fact explaining why, among humans, it is far more likely to be males than females who go about confusing sexuality with aggression. That it's got something to do with this weird role of the amygdala in male sexual motivation, male sexual arousal. What else? OK. Males have penises, thus they're the only ones who can have penile erections, and to do that, autonomic nervous system. And what we will hear about, what you already heard about in the introduction to the autonomic nervous system, but also in the zebra's book is that whole business in order to manage that, to pull that off initially, it is parasympathetic nervous system that establishes the erection. The process of arousal involves the transition to sympathetic. Full blast sympathetic nervous system needed for ejaculation, that's what all of that is about. Exclusive to males. But then it turns out it's not exclusive to males because it's virtually the exact same physiology underlying clitoral erections in females, the same exact sort of thing, which, of course, brings up the dangerous possibility of another spandrel in our laps here in terms of making sense of that. I didn't say that just now. Did I say spandrels in your lap? OK. Bringing up that possibility that is not specific to male physiology. What is specific, of course, is stuff that's going on with penises in terms of blood flow. There's generally a dichotomy between species, between whether or not males get vascular erections or muscular erections. Vascular erections, you increase blood flow into the penis, and you stop it from going out the other end. And thus, you get a vascular-driven engorgement as DH Lawrence would no doubt have described it. Alternatively, in lots of species there are muscular erections. There is a muscle, for example, found in rodent penises called the erector [? levae ?] muscle-- well, that's not too surprising that it's called that-- and a whole bunch of cell bodied neurons in the spinal cord responsible for pulling up the sail or whatever it is you do there. And what you get are differences in general. The muscular-driven erections occur a lot faster. The vascular ones, the hemodynamic ones, last a lot longer. Take your pick, but it's essentially the exact same autonomic physiology in both cases. Finally, one other thing, a factoid, a useful one we heard last week, which is insofar as there is very similar physiology to orgasms in both sexes, there is that difference in recovery time, how long it takes for the sympathetic nervous system to go back to baseline post orgasm. And on the average, a substantial sex difference in that. Yes, everybody managed to guess it last week, which direction it went. A far slower recovery time in females than in males. In terms of underlying neurobiology, something that was a major finding in the field were brain regions that differed in size depending on your gender, including in humans. And this ushered in a whole world of sexual dimorphism in the brain. And there have now been shown to be all sorts of brain regions where, on the average, you get differences in the size of nuclei. You get differences in the number of axons going through a bundle of fiber, all of that. And we will hear about some more of those down the line. But the one that has gotten the most attention in terms of sexual behavior is a cluster of tiny nuclei in the hypothalamus called the INAH cluster, the Interstitial Nucleus of the Anterior Hypothalamus. Do not write down what that stands for. But it's a little nucleus in there there, a little subset of neuronal cell bodies, where you get a very substantial sex difference in the size of this area, where on the average, it is about twice the size in men as in women. And back to the other week's rant about statistical significance versus magnitude, this is a big effect. It is almost, almost in the range where you can identify the sex of somebody by looking at the size of this nucleus in their brain post-mortem. In rodents, you pretty much can. A very reliable two-fold difference, males larger than females. As we'll hear in a while, one really interesting exception to that. OK. So either some areas of the brain that are preferentially involved and activated by sexual behavior, depending on your gender, or regions that differ in size substantially by your gender. But then, at the end of the day, there's all sorts of things that are absolutely in common. Again, the physiology of orgasm, exactly the same. What clinically the picture is is males having problems with the whole system. The problem tends to be too rapid of a transition from parasympathetic to sympathetic. In other words, the world of premature ejaculation. The more typical medical problem in women is failure of the transition from parasympathetic to sympathetic, inability to reach orgasm. And it is, of course, a huge social, cultural, political, philosophical argument whether that counts as a pathology or normal human variability. I'm not going anywhere near that one. But nonetheless, that is the more common pattern. Neurobiology that's absolutely in common between the sexes, which is all of that stuff at the very beginning of why are they in such a rush at end, the neurobiology of pleasure, and the neurobiology of reward, and of anticipation. And this is this whole world of-- as we know already-- dopamine. The role of dopamine in sexual behavior is virtually identical in both sexes, which is to say it plays a huge role. You find circumstances where you deplete dopamine from the relevant brain regions-- back to last Friday-- limbic system. You remember that ventral tegmental area, which sends that big dopaminergic projection to the nucleus accumbens, which then passes it on to all sorts of places in the brain. Deplete that pathway of dopamine, and you're not going to get a whole lot of interest in sexual behavior. You're not going to get a whole lot of libido proceptivity. What's the classic circumstance where you see depletion of dopamine there and loss of proceptive libido? Clinical depression. That's one of the defining symptoms of depression amid the various numerous forms of pleasure that go down the tubes. Loss of sexual interest, one of the defining symptoms. So the dopamine system. The general term given for that is the mesolimbic dopamine system to distinguish it from some of the other ways that dopamine is used in the brain. The mesolimbic dopamine system is absolutely central to the reinforcing aspects of sexual behavior. So what's the evidence for that? First off, back to that distinction that I think I brought up last week-- I wasn't paying attention-- but I think I talked about, which is the dopamine system there is not so much about reward, it's about the anticipation of reward. Did I talk about that in monkeys pressing levers? Yes. OK. I should probably read the extended notes at some point or look at the film of this. OK. What you see there is the dopamine is about the anticipation of. And the dopamine, as we heard, is about also fueling the behaviors needed to achieve the reward. Dopamine in this mesolimbic pathway as driving goal-directed behavior. And that is certainly the case with sexual behavior. By now, there is a whole literature involving humans where you stick them in brain scanners and you do something or other sexually arousing or interesting or something or other to them. And then you see what parts of the brain activate. And it's these dopamine pathways consistently way up there. Showing just how subtle this can be, how's this? You take men-- there's been a whole literature by now showing that you present people in brain scanners with pornography. And that must have been a really interesting in human subjects release form you worked out. But showing in both sexes, what you tend to see is activation of dopaminergic regions. We will hear in a little while a sex difference in that domain that will probably not surprise anyone. But how's this for subtle? You take a guy, and you show him the picture of someone of the opposite sex if he is heterosexual. And you show him the picture of this individual. And if it is someone who he assesses as being attractive, you don't necessarily get this dopaminergic pathway to activate. It depends. What this study showed was if the person is making what would pass for eye contact, if they were looking straight out, the dopamine system activates. And if they're looking elsewhere, it doesn't activate. How's that for a classic male sort of responsiveness? If it looks as if this attractive person is looking at you, it activates. Even more distressingly from this study, when you show men-- on the average, blah, blah-- pictures of women who they would rate as being unattractive, it's when they're looking away that the dopamine system activates. Oh my god! What is going on here? This is pitiful. What also has been shown is the exact same eye contact phenomenon of gay men looking at pictures of attractive men. Another theme we're going to see over and over, which is sexual orientation being pretty much trivial in terms of how it influences some of this neurobiology. Just switch the gender of the other individual, and it works exactly the same. Now when you look at this business about dopamine rising in anticipation of a reward rather than a response to the reward itself, it brings up one of the-- it doesn't bring that up-- it brings up one of the all-time interesting studies that was published about a decade ago. OK. So the paradigm I described last week, you put on the light, which tells the monkey that, OK, we're starting one of those sessions where if you press the levers adequately, you will get a reward. And they now carry out this behavior. And as a result, they get the reward here. And as we saw, dopamine doesn't go up after the reward. It goes up at this point. This is the I how to do this. This is going to be great. This is terrific. Here's where you get the rise in dopamine. This is not only the anticipation, but if you don't have this rise, you don't get the behavior, the goal-directed behavior. Now in this brilliant study, what they did was transition from a paradigm where, OK, the monkey presses the lever 10 times and gets the reward. Now what you do is the monkey works, and it gets the reward only half the time. It gets only a 50% reward rate unpredictably. And what happens to dopamine? OK. Got your choice. What's your vote? It doesn't rise as much. It rises the exact same amount. It rises even higher. OK. You guys all understand anticipation and goal-- it does this. It's one of the biggest rises you will find in dopamine in the brain short of cocaine. What have you just introduced into there? This is, I'm all over it. I know how this works. This is going to be great. I have mastery and control. I am the captain of my own lever pressing. This is all about that. What's this about? This is what dopamine does when you've introduced the word maybe into the equation. And that is incredibly reinforcing. And people will work like mad in contexts of maybe far more so than when they work in contexts of certainty. Psychologists have known this forever. This is intermittent reinforcement. You never get more behavior out of an organism than when you have introduced a maybe into it. And part of the brilliance of this study was what they then did. Now animals either got reward 25% of the time or 75% of the time. On a certain level, these are diametrically opposite manipulations. In one, you're getting more rewards. In the other, you're getting less. What's the thing they have in common? They both had smaller maybes than the 50% version. And what you see is it would look like this. 100%, 25% or 75%, 50% maximizing the maybe. And one of the most brilliant things that various social engineers do with humans is convince people that there's a 50% maybe when it is not 50% in the slightest. That's what Las Vegas is about. That's an entire world of very smart psychologists making people think in circumstances where there's like one tenth of 1% of a maybe going on there that is actually a 50%. And when you do that, you get dopamine like crazy, and you get goal-directed behavior as a result. Really, really powerful. And this is so strongly the case that this explains an extremely cynical thing that a guy I knew in my dorm back when used to say all the time. How's this for like a dispirited view of what life is like, but possibly absolutely accurate, which is, a relationship is the price you pay for the anticipation of it. How's that for a grim worldview? Go figure. This guy had-- what a string of disastrous relationships. But what you see here is introduce a maybe, and it is very, very powerful. One final piece of the dopamine system here that is pertinent, which is, as you might expect from all of our molecular biology stuff, there's all sorts of different dopamine receptor subtypes. And two of them are pertinent to this world of sexual behavior and reward, what is called the shockingly the D1 dopamine receptor and the D2 dopamine receptor. And what studies show is in monogamous species, what happens is right after mating, when a pair bond is first formed, the second that's over with, levels of the D2 receptor go way down. You down regulate the levels of the receptor, and you up regulate the levels of the D1 receptor. What's that about? If you drive down the D2 levels before they even mate, they don't form a pair bond. If you prevent the decline in the D2's after they've mated and pair bonded or if you prevent the rise in the D1's, they'll pair bond. And then, 8 and 1/2 minutes later, they will go and pair bond with somebody else. The D2's seem to mediate the rewarding anticipatory aspects of pair bonding. The D1's, on a certain rodential level, seem to mediate the pleasure of the monogamous, the truly monogamous features of the pair bond. So a very interesting interaction between the two. OK. One last thing about dopamine, and this one is like even more depressing than relationships are the price you pay. This was a study which was really like someday may come to haunt you majorly. And in this study, what they did-- it was another one of those brain imaging study ones. And what they did was they took people in two categories. In both cases, these are people who had found their beloved, their beloved, the person who was their soul mate, the person in whose arms they were going to die someday, the person. And they divided it between these two groups. One was a group where they had known the person in whose arms they were going to die for like 2 and 1/2 weeks. And the other is when they had been together for more than five years. So you put somebody in the brain scanner, and you start flashing up at speed, subliminal speeds, of pictures of individuals they know. Important control in the study. And embedded in there is a picture of their beloved. And suddenly, somewhere along the way, up flashes the picture of their beloved. Be in a short-term relationship and the dopamine system goes crazy and activates like mad. Now, you come back five years later into that same relationship with the beloved, and you do the exact same thing. And you flash up their picture, and the dopamine system doesn't activate. What activates instead was that anterior cingulate thing we heard about on Friday having to do with empathy, and comfort, and all of that. In other words, what we see here is the neurochemical transition from one's beloved from causing your blood to run scalding hot to your beloved being like a comfortable old armchair. This is one depressing study. So let's take a five-minute break to contemplate that one. OK. And then we will resume. Lots of good questions just now during the break. Disappointingly few along the lines of, I've got a friend who. So not a bunch of those. But let's see. A number of questions. First one, can I repeat what I said about the D1 and the D2 receptors? OK. These are different types of receptors for dopamine. In other words, they all respond to the same neurotransmitter dopamine, but in different ways. And these receptors are found on different neuron types. So you're getting into all sorts of different pathways. What you see is, in rodents, in pair bonding rodents, they better have elevated levels, they better have D2 receptors on neurons being fed by this mesolimbic reward dopamine pathway. They have to have D2 receptors to form the pair bond for the attachment to occur. The second that happens, you need to have low levels of D2 and high levels of D1 to remain faithful in your pair bonded relationship. So what you see in these voles is right after the pair bond occurs, there's down regulation of the D2's and up regulation of the D1's. And if you prevent that from happening, the pair bond that's been formed does not prove lasting. So that's what I was saying there. Somebody brought up the great question, which I was going to say something about and forgot, which is, well, how about like the D2 D1 ratios in humans and their sexual behavior stuff? And there's been one study showing that a higher ratio of D2 to D1 predicts more stable relationships. Small effect. Not replicated yet. But nonetheless, that's kind of interesting. So on a certain level then, D2 seems to be required, at least shown in rodents. Who knows about us? D2 is about the formation of the attachment. D1 is about the maintenance of it, the faithfulness of it, if you will. Next, somebody bringing up the issue in terms of female orgasm. Maybe what female orgasm is about is a mate selection mechanism, as in individuals who increase your likelihood of having orgasms are ones you are more likely to lower your D2 receptors for. But the one problem with that one that makes wonderful sense-- what the studies tend to show though is the likelihood of orgasm is much more a function of who the female is than who the male is that they are with, arguing against that. Let's see. Finally-- no. Not that. OK. So we already covered that. And there was one additional question. OK. So what's the driving force in terms of the proximal reinforcing pleasurable aspects of sexual behavior? What's up with why only some species-- us predominantly-- have non-reproductive sex, can have sex all the time, versus other species that only do for reproduction? What that means is in other species, the endocrinology of ovulation is the thing that makes sex pleasurable. And we will see shortly what that's about. In females, it's the hormones associated with ovulation that sensitize various tactile receptors to respond in ways that mediate proximal pleasure. And in males, it's the female giving off, for example, the right pheromones, the right releasing stimuli, driven by the right hormone levels that constitute the proximal signal of pleasurable anticipation. And what you find in humans is it doesn't work that way. You do not need, for example, in women the elevated levels of estrogen typical of ovulation in order to have tactile responsiveness to sexually arousing stimuli. Stay tuned though. It's easier though when estrogen levels are higher. OK. Final brain region relevant to all of this is the frontal cortex. We already got a first pass at the frontal cortex last week. And frontal cortex regulating your behavior, impulse control, all that sort of thing, gratification postponement-- this plays a large role in sexual behavior. What's the easy immediate explanation that one can come up with, what the frontal cortex does is it makes you be appropriate in your sexual behavior. It teaches you the appropriate context. It teaches you what aspects of proceptive sexual behavior is not a good idea. It keeps you from doing things you would regret vastly afterward. That's a very easy version of it. And commensurate with that, you see lots of circumstances of individuals with frontal cortical damage doing highly inappropriate sexual behavior. One example of it, and one of those horrifying things that can happen-- this was a case that actually happened in a nursing home in Martinez in the East Bay a number of years ago. This was a man in his 80s who had had stroke damage to his frontal cortex, who was found to have raped a woman there, another 80-year-old with Alzheimer's disease. Damage the frontal cortex, and all sorts of the, "this is not sexual behavior that you do" constraints go down the tubes. Just as importantly though, what the frontal cortex, with all of it's giving you the discipline to do the right thing, some of the time, what that takes the form of is getting you to do proceptive sexual behavior. For example, you are trying to do some courtship of some other antlered ungulate that you are courting. And this is terrifying because there is another individual challenging you. And there's the frontal cortex that is getting you to carry out those sexual behaviors to that point, even if it is a terrifying circumstance. Nonetheless, what the frontal cortex mostly is about is reigning in sexual behavior. It's not changing the fixed action patterns of sex. It's changing the context in which the fixed action patterns occur. So now, we are ready to look at one more feature of the neurobiology, which is when somebody is having sex, what hormonal responses are triggered? Notice this is not here. This is not what hormones have to do with bringing about sexual behavior. This is, what are the hormonal responses to sexual behavior? Starting off in females, including human females, having sex increases secretion of progesterone-derived hormones. And that has something to do with reinforcing the pleasure. Interestingly, in females the world over, having sex increases the level of testosterone-related hormones in the bloodstream, androgens. Women, females, generate androgens maybe 5% the levels you see in males. And they come out of the adrenal glands. And they seem to play a very central role in mediating sexual motivation, sexual arousal, in females. How is that's shown? Obvious experimental studies with lab rats. How is that's shown in humans? When women have any of a number of types of diseases where you have to take out the adrenal glands, sexual motivation, sexual arousal, goes down. Give them replacement androgens, and sexual arousal, sexual proceptivity, returns, so androgens there playing a role. But probably most importantly, in terms of hormones triggered by sexual behavior in females, is the release of oxytocin. Oxytocin is really interesting. We've heard about oxytocin twice already. One, is when it's coming out of the posterior pituitary. And the second is that minor business the other day, last Wednesday, of oxytocin being another one of those hypothalamic hormones that helps to release ACTH from the pituitary. Remember, it doesn't directly release. It's a modulator, a CRH action, if-then clause, et cetera. But those are the two ways we've heard about oxytocin. Oxytocin also works in the brain as a neurotransmitter and neuromodulator. And what does it do there? It appears to play a very central role in forming attachments, a very central role in forming of pair bonds. And it, along with dopamine and the D2 receptors, are critical for female voles of monogamous species to form pair bonds. Female humans, when having sex, secrete lots of oxytocin and activate oxytocin pathways in the brain. And it appears to play a role in the formation of attachment. Interestingly-- how's this-- a whole body of research now showing that if you introduce oxytocin into the brains of humans experimentally, they become more trusting. Amazing body of research where you take aerosolized oxytocin and you spritz it up people's noses. And what they showed in the studies were, number one, one type of study. You then play a clip of somebody making an argument for some stance in some debate, and people believe the person more. They find their argument more convincing. They trust the person more. Or the other version that's been shown is you spritz oxytocin up the noses of people, and you make them more cooperative in their game theory play, the ways in which they go about playing prisoner's dilemma and other games we will hear about later on as well. This has given rise to a whole new field-- I kid you not-- called neuromarketing. The notion that if only you could spritz oxytocin up the noses of people right before your television ad comes on, they're going to believe you when you say it will make you happy to buy our thing. And they will fall for it. There are actually neuroendocrinologists making a living now selling their wares to neuromarketing people-- self-proclaimed ones. No doubt they are spritzing oxytocin up the noses of those capitalists to get them to hire them to do this. But oxytocin playing a role in this. So that's kind of interesting. Because what's oxytocin mostly doing in the body? The vast majority of oxytocin is not this stuff up in the brain in these pathways, some of them impinging on dopamine-releasing neurons. The vast majority is not the oxytocin sitting there in the hypothalamus doing something or other to ACTH in the pituitary. The vast majority is this stuff coming out of the posterior pituitary. And what does oxytocin have to do there? It has to do with milk letdown. It has to do with nursing. And suddenly, instead, it's playing a role in forming sexual pair bonding. And the argument is made that attachment, monogamous attachment, sexual attachment, is in some way, evolutionarily a descendent of the neurobiology of mother-offspring attachment. That that's where it is originally being driven by. So oxytocin playing a role there as well. Meanwhile, over at the male end of things, up go testosterone levels during sex. In a surprisingly linear way, the more sexual behavior in a male, the higher testosterone levels are found afterward. Critically, critically-- stay tuned for a little while-- these are elevations of testosterone in response to sexual behavior. As we'll see, the evidence that high testosterone levels make males more sexually active is basically nonexistent. So critical, critical proviso here. This is sex increasing testosterone secretion, not the other way around. What else? Meanwhile, back to the posterior pituitary. Was that a question? No. OK. That was a head scratch. OK. Back to the posterior pituitary. The other hormone coming out from there, vasopressin. And oxytocin is to females as vasopressin is to males. And we've already heard something about this back in the molecular genetics stuff in those if-then clauses and unlikely ways in which you get mutations. vasopressin, vasopressin also is found in the nervous system, where it serves as neuromodulatory role. And vasopressin is critical for forming a pair bond. Back to that business about when you look at monogamous versus polygamist species, what's going on? What you have uniquely in the monogamous species is expression of the vasopressin receptor gene on neurons that released dopamine. In other words, male secrete vasopressin. And if you are of a species where vasopressin now goes and stimulates dopamine neurons, you decide you really, really, really liked having sex with this other vole. And you come back for more. And that's the driving force on the formation of the pair bond. Incredible studies showing that if you take male voles from the polygamist species and, due to gene transfer techniques-- I've mentioned the study already-- but you now stick vasopressin receptors into those dopamine neurons, those polygamist males now become pair bonding males. They now become monogamous. So really interesting. What you then see in these studies is you look at these monogamous species where the males have vasopressin receptors on these dopamine neurons, and you look at individual males, and the ones who have more receptors there are forming pair bonds faster. It takes fewer rounds of mating with a female to form a pair bond. So what about primates? So you start off looking at two different primate species, one pair bonding, marmoset monkeys, New World marmoset monkeys, and then one classic tournament species, polygamous primate species, rhesus monkeys. And what you see is you've got the variant, the monogamous vole, vasopressin receptor gene variant in the pair bonding monkey species, in the marmosets. And you get the polygamous version of the gene in the rhesus monkeys. So it maps on there as well. So the more of this receptor in these dopamine pathways within monogamous species, the more rapidly they form a pair bond. And what you see is differences in the mere presence of them in comparing pair bonding versus non-pair bonding rodents and monkeys. So how about humans? First thing that comes up is, among the apes, you also find the two variants, the monogamous vole variant of the vasopressin receptor gene and the polygamous male variant. So what species do you see it in? In chimpanzees, you see the polygamous vole species version. That makes lots of sense. They are a major polygamist species in their behavior. But then, this beautiful dichotomy comes crashing down when you see that you've got the monogamous gene version in bonobos. And as we will hear about in a while, bonobos are the most hyperpolygamously, hyper varietyish sexually behaving organisms on the entire planet. They are as far as you could get from a monogamous species as you can ever ask for, if you ask for such things. And you've got the wrong type of vasopressin receptor gene. Whatever is going on, it's more complicated than the have the version that winds up on the dopamine neurons and you are going to have 50th wedding anniversaries if you are a vole or you are a marmoset. And it's got to be more complicated than that. So how about humans? And what you see is not explicitly as much genetic variability as some people having the monogamous vole version and some people have the polygamist. But nonetheless, you get variation. The gene basically is about halfway in between. Whoa. We keep having that theme over and over, all these different ways of looking at body size, and sexual dimorphism, and imprinted genes, and all that stuff. And humans keep winding up being about a halfway between a classic monogamous pair bonder and a classic polygamist tournament species. So the basic human version of it is somewhere in between. But you get variations. You've got genetic variations that either look a little bit more like the monogamous vole version or the polygamous vole version. And what studies have now shown-- two different studies independently showing this-- have the monogamous vole version. And with the small effect, you are more likely to get married, you are more likely to remain married, and both you and your partner are more likely to rate the marriage as stable and happy. That's kind of interesting. Finally, in terms of the role of vasopressin, in terms of attachment in males, all of that, and in terms of social connectiveness, a large body of studies now have shown mutations in the vasopressin receptor gene. OK. Anybody want to guess what disorder you find it? I put that in the extended notes, haven't I? People have read it already. I know, I fell for it last time. I am not falling for that stupid trick again of asking you to prove. He sniffed some oxytocin. So does anybody want to guess which the-- OK. What do you now all know is there's now been a lot of demonstrations of mutations in the vasopressin receptor gene in family pedigrees with autism, a disease of very, very little attachment to other humans. So we've got written all over the place here some sort of role of oxytocin in female attachment formation and vasopressin. And we've already gotten interesting hints that this applies to humans. And we've already gotten interesting hints that individual differences in the molecular biology of these genes predicts something about individual differences in the stability of relationships in humans. OK. So everything we've been hearing about with the neurobiology-- and we're still living in this part, in this bucket here temporarily-- has been built around heterosexual relationships. What's known about the neurobiology of sexual orientation? What has been found is most strikingly one landmark study, one that got on the cover of Time magazine, one that had a gigantic, gigantic impact, which was looking back at that hypothalamic nucleus, that INAH, I-N-A-H. Yes. Where what we saw before was very reliably on the average, men, their size of it is about twice the size as in women. And in other species, males about twice the size as in females. And a study was done in the late 80s by a neuroanatomist names Simon LeVay. LeVay is one of the all-time great neuroanatomists. He was trained by Hubel and Wiesel, was a professor at Harvard Med School for a while before moving to the Salk Institute. And what he did was look post-mortem at the brains of a bunch of individuals where he knew the sexual orientation, men. And what he showed was gay men, on the average, had this nucleus on the average was half the size that you saw in heterosexual men. On the average, it was about the same size as you saw in heterosexual women. Amazing landmark study. Everybody learned about the homosexual brain from this study. Hugely widely reported. And this is kind of interesting. OK. What's interesting about it? First off, the question you need to ask is how much variability? Fair amount. It wasn't all that reliable of a difference. On the average, it was about a half the size. Next thing you would want to know is, has anybody replicated it since then? Yes. Next thing you would want to know is, where did LeVay get the brains from? And these were predominantly from gay men who had died of AIDS. Is that a confound? Is that going to perhaps atrophy this part of the brain? Nobody knows. So that remains as a caveat in that study. What was striking though was everybody learned about this finding. This became famous. LeVay became extremely famous for this. And what was also very interesting about it was the political context of this finding. A few years before that, another group had reported another difference in the hypothalamus based on sexual orientation. And this was-- there it is. And what you found was in this part of the brain, on the average, it would tend be bigger in women than in men. And what these guys reported was in gay men, it tended to be bigger than in straight men. What was puzzling about the study was this was a part of a hypothalamus having to do with regulation of your kidneys. And it was totally ignored and completely bizarre, except there was one thing that was done with it, which was this was viewed as a totally offensive study in the gay community. This was viewed as an attempt by scientists to pathologize sexual orientation, to say, you see, we found something wrong in the brains of homosexual men. This part of the brain is bigger than it should be. It's bigger than it's supposed to be. There's probably two reasons why that occurred. The first one was that the scientists who did it were straight. And the second reason being that they were a Dutch group. And I am willing to bet unconsciously there was something central European Nazi echoes of Germanic sounding authors producing this finding, which there is no shortage of history in the gay community for being skittish about Nazi notions of what normality is in human behavior and human brains. This finding got widely condemned in the gay community. Out came LeVay with his finding, and he became the most beloved neuroanatomist ever in the history of gay communities. One probably important feature reason for it is that LeVay was gay, very openly so. Another reason was the part of the brain he found made sense. It had something to do with sexual behavior as opposed to the totally puzzling thing. So what was this about? Very interestingly, the explanation almost certainly is that this was the part of the hypothalamus next door to the area that LeVay studied. And if this part were smaller in gay men simply because of just physical constraints stuff, this part could get bigger in gay men. Because this one was taking up less area. That's probably what was going on. What's really fascinating though is the political context that this research was done in. And this was that first group, the senior author of it, a man named Dick Schwab. And he got death threats because of that study reporting, ooh, here is-- easily interpreted as-- something wrong in the brains of gay man. And Simon LeVay became the hero in the community. This was utterly embraced by the community. Because in large part, how it was interpreted as, this is biology. This isn't choice. This is biology. Look at this. This is ridiculous, us saying, oh my god. If we have gay teachers in the classroom, we will turn the Boy Scouts of America into a gay men. Oh my god, if we have blue-eyed teachers in the classroom-- the usual argument that this is absolute gibberish to demonize sexual orientation as a choice. Look at this. There's a neurobiology of it. And this was sufficiently that I've seen people in the Castro District in San Francisco-- this has disappeared somewhat, showing the half life of neuroanatomical knowledge. But during that time, people in the Castro District up in the city there, which is a very gay community, I have seen people with t-shirts saying-- the other term people never really wanted to embrace this INAH, so its nickname was the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the hypothalamus. And I have seen people with t-shirts saying, the only small thing about me is my sexually dimorphic nucleus. They were actually selling t-shirts that would say this during gay pride parades around 1990 or so. Isn't it great when people learn neuroanatomy out in the general public there? OK. So an interesting brain difference confounded by people who died of AIDS. It's still not clear what that means in terms of possibly negating the finding at least independently replicated. Fascinating piece of not science, but the political context of science. This politically incorrect to an extreme, this very, very widely embraced. One interesting thing is that paper by LeVay of was published in the journal Science. Again, constantly mentioned as probably the most influential science journal in this country. And it was published-- what year was it? '88. '88, '92, '90? OK. I'm getting the year wrong. But it was just before the Clinton election against Bush where one of the big issues was gays in the military. That was the first thing that Clinton turned to after he was elected. I happen to know the man who is the editor of Science at the time. And they timed the publication. That paper came out in late October of that year. And they held it for that time because they knew that this was going to be an issue in the election, which was kind of cool that they did that, although some people may disagree. OK. But we hurtle on. So what other biological, neurobiological differences, as a function of sexual orientation? Another one that comes through over and over and over again, which-- what do you make of this-- is apparently there is a reliable gender difference in the length of the second finger versus the fourth finger, the ratio of the two. And just to show how bi-- whoa, did a lot of hands just go up in this auditorium. And just to show how biologically compelling the explanation is for it, I don't actually remember which like-- who's got the greater four to two ratio, which sex, or whatever. But it is a very-- now people are checking each other's hands. OK. The first wave of-- and now all this chimp hand inspection stuff happening in here. And what has been shown quite reliably since then is, on the average, gay men tend to have the finger length ratio of straight women rather than of straight men. A small effect there. Another even more bizarre finding, which is there is something called the autoacoustic reflex. And what that is is if you sit there and plug your ears up with your fingers, you will hear a noise that is just coming from the intrinsic vibration of something right there in your ears, and that's the autoacoustic reflex generating some low Hertz sound in there. And the rate of the oscillation differs by sex in humans, which no doubt explains everything about the tragic wars of the sexes and why people just don't understand each other by gender because of their ears vibrating at different speeds. But what these studies have also shown was gay men having the autoacoustic reflexive vibratory speed more typical of straight women than straight men. Again, a very small effect. What are all of these about? The assumptions are it's got to do with something with prenatal hormone environment. Stay tuned. We will be coming back to this. Now somewhere in there you may ask, OK, well what about the neurobiology of sexual orientation in women? Vastly smaller literature. Far, far less studied. What has been shown so far are only two endpoints. One is the same deal with the fourth to second finger ratio. On the average, gay women have the ratio more typical of straight men than straight women. The other thing that's been shown is the same autoacoustic reflex thingy going on there. Final realm of neurobiology, rather than issues of gay versus straight, what is the neurobiology of transsexuality? And that used to be considered to be purely a domain of psychopathology. If being gay used to be a certifiable psychiatric disorder-- up until the early 1970s, the American Psychiatric Association in their textbook, the Diagnostic Statistical Manual, you could be psychiatrically certified as ill. A psychiatric disorder was being homosexual or lesbian. And then in what had to have been one of the more all-time blow out committee meetings ever, they decided that, no, actually it's not a psychiatric disorder. And overnight, about 40 million Americans were cured of a psychiatric disease. The notion of transsexuality as a psychiatric disorder has had much, much longer shelf life. What's the neurobiology of that? To date, there have been a handful of studies, and they show essentially the same thing-- really, really interesting. Another region of the brain that shows a sex difference in its average size-- don't even worry about the name of this. It's called the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. It's where the amygdala begins to send its projection into the hypothalamus. Another one to those gender differences. There is one type of neuron in there with a certain type of neurotransmitter, where very, very reliably it is about twice the size in males than in females. Sufficiently so that even in human brains, you could pretty confidently determine the sex of somebody by seeing the number of these neurons. You'll see, I'm not even saying the name of the neurotransmitter. It's irrelevant. It's just another one of those differences, a dimorphism in a region of the brain, a really, really reliable one. And this was a study done by some superb neuroanatomists looking at transsexuals. And what they showed was very interesting, which was very, very reliably and a very powerful effect. What you would see in their large sample size of transsexuals brains post-mortem was people would have this part of the brain, the size not of their sex that they were born with, but rather of the sex they insisted they always actually were. Wow. Immediate questions one must ask. OK. Well, maybe this is due to the fact that when people change gender, transsexual procedures, there's a whole lot of hormones involved. And maybe that's doing something to this part of the brain. Critical control that they had was this was looking both at transsexuals who had made gender changes and those who went to their death bed saying this is not the sex that I am, I got the wrong body, but never made the change. It wasn't a function of having actually gone through the transition and the endocrine manipulations with it. Another control they had, which was looking at men who would get a certain type of testicular cancer where they would have to be treated with certain feminizing hormones. In other words, very similar to some of the endocrine treatments of male-to-female transgendered individuals. And post-mortem, you didn't see the changes there. It has nothing to do with the hormones. It had to do with the person insisting from day one that they got the wrong body. And this was a landmark study, fabulously well done and controlled, and replicated once since then showing that what transsexualism used to be thought of is that people who think that they're a different gender than they actually are. What this study suggests is what transsexualism is about is people who got the wrong gendered body. And these are people who are chromosomally of one sex. In terms of their gonads, they're of that sex. In terms of their hormones, they're of that sex. In terms of their genitalia and their secondary sexual characteristics, they are of that sex. But they're insisting, that's not who I really am. This part of the brain agrees with them. Also very interestingly that study was done by the same Dutch scientists who did this one. Again, this is very complex terrain in terms of what these things wind up implicating. Interestingly, that study was published right around the time that the city of San Francisco did something very cool, which was for city employees now, medical insurance will cover transgender operations. However, there is no evidence that the obscure endocrine journal published out of Latvia or something did that like the afternoon before the San Francisco commissioners had their meeting on that one. But nonetheless, this is a subject with all sorts of realms of implications. One additional study about transsexualism. OK. How many of you know about Phantom Limb Syndrome? OK. You are a guy with a penis, and you get a certain type of penile cancer. And what's often done is your penis is excised. It is cut off. And about 60% of men who have had to have their penises removed because of cancer there wind up getting phantom penile sensations, which I don't want to know about. What you see though is when you take transgendered individuals who go from male to female, in other words, as part of it having their penises removed, 0% rate of penile phantom sensation. Suggestion being that there is something much more "normal" in that case than when a penis is being removed for cancer, a whole new area of research, very novel, very challenging. OK. So this has giving us a sense now of this bucket. And we are now ready to move on to, what in the environment releases some of these fixed action patterns of sexual behavior? What in the environment is doing this or that to the medial preoptic area or the amygdala or vasopressin receptor levels or any such thing. What are the sensory triggers for the neurobiology of sexual behavior? OK. Right off, what is obvious is we are in ethologyville here. It's going to depend on the species which sensory modality is most important. And this is, once again, the crushing of the limbic system equals nose-brain concept. Limbic system equals nose-brain if you are a rat. It's, once again, going to be interviewing an animal in its own language. You've got species where the releasing stimuli are all visual. And we've heard one example of that already, which were the pathetic male turkeys getting faked out by the Styrofoam female turkeys with the feathers pointing the wrong way. Visual stimuli. Other species are quite visual as well. Primates, non-human primates, monkeys, for example. And what studies have shown is-- how's this for remarkable? OK. Here we have-- nah, forget it. OK. You will take a rhesus monkey, a rhesus monkey from a social group. And he's sitting there, and he can lever press for various rewards. And he will press a lever a certain number of times to get some juice as a reward. He will press a lever a certain number of times to see a high ranking male from his social group, no doubt to keep an eye on the guy. He will not lever press to see a male who is lower ranking than him, but he will lever the most to see pictures of female rhesus monkeys who are in heat. Whoa! Is that weird or what? And the bigger the estrus swelling on the female, the more levered pressing the male will do to be able to see this. So that's kind of interesting. And this close relative of ours, in terms of visual stimuli. What is also known is humans are highly visual in their sexual responsiveness as well. Visual stimuli as releasing stimuli. What is no surprise whatsoever is on the average, males are more responsive to visual releasing stimuli than are females among humans. And this has been shown in various ways. For example, now studies using brain imaging showing that for visually sexually arousing material that not only are men on the average subjectively more responsive, but you get more of an activation of the dopamine pathways in men than in women. What's interesting also is that in men, you uniquely get activation of that area of the amygdala as well. And again, that weird world of the structure of the brain heavily involved in aggression also being involved something about male sexual motivation. What else? Then there, of course, is the world of tactile stimulation. And what you've got is a whole domain where, not surprisingly, stimulate the right tactile receptors, and it is sexually arousing. Are they sure? Have they done enough research on this? And you will activate dopamine regions. All of that making perfect sense. What also makes perfect sense is some types of tactile receptors in some part of the body activate dopamine more than other types. We are now in the whole world of erogenous zones and that whole deal. What is also clear is that these receptors, these tactile receptors, their responsiveness to stimuli will change depending on your hormone levels. And what you see is in women, tactile responsiveness, the extent to which tactile stimulation of skin throughout the body, but especially of the genitals, tactile stimulation evokes more dopamine activation when somebody is ovulating. In other words, at ovulation, women's skin is more sensitive to sexually arousing touch. In men, it requires testosterone. Men who are castrated, tactile responsiveness to stimuli goes down in terms of finding them pleasurable, sexually arousing. Final domain of tactile stuff, the specialized version we've heard of already, that lordosis reflex business. Again, that's a spinal reflex, but this is not a spinal reflex of bopping somebody on the knee and their leg goes flying out. This is spinal reflex where you only get the lordotic arch backing in females-- arch backing? Back arching. OK. You don't get either, but you especially don't get the arch backing when you don't have elevated estrogen levels. Only when females are ovulating are those tactile receptors sensitive to pressure on the flanks of the rear end, and out comes the reflex there. So tactile stimulation. At the end of the day, though, without question, as agreed upon by every scientist on earth, the coolest sensory modality for sexual release stimuli are olfactory cues, pheromones. And thus, we enter the magnificent wonderful world of pheromonal communication and pheromonal sexual arousal. All sorts of interesting findings there. First, at the end of generating pheromones that are sexually arousing. What is required in both species-- in both species? Whoa. That was an interesting slip. What's required in both sexes-- OK. What's required in both kinds is the right hormones into order to generate pheromones. Sexually arousing pheromones in all the different species look-- that's where the species part was coming into that sentence. What you see is males do not generate sexually arousing pheromones if they lack testosterone levels. Ovariectomized females, women, rats, monkeys, et cetera, who have had their ovaries removed do not produce pheromones that are sexually arousing. What that of course brings up is, what are some of the chemical constituents of pheromones? And this is very interesting because it brings up another one of those weird domains of neuromarketing, pheromones that have sexually arousing components. There's all sorts of fatty acids that play a role in that. But a lot of what is sexually arousing about pheromones in different species are breakdown products of sex hormones. Breakdown products of androgens in males, of estrogens in females. And that winds of providing some of these sexually arousing aspects of those odors. What does that immediately tell you? Your olfactory receptors have all sorts of receptors there that could pick up on remnants of testosterone and estrogen, things of that sort. That makes a lot of sense. What doesn't make any sense at all is the following finding, which is perfume. Perfume, in its classic form, is made out of the sweat of various animals. OK. Except we're going to get into even worse domain here, which is perfumes traditionally, before getting the synthetic versions, were typically made from the sweat of male animals. Hm. What's that about? Musk. Things of that sort. Chanel No. 5 is made from the sweat of whipped male Abyssinian cats. I kid you not. And this even produced protests some years ago, animal rights groups, about how we should not be perfuming ourselves with the sweat of whipped male Abyssinian cats. Suddenly, you've got a real puzzle. Along comes synthetic perfumes, and the majority of them are made of synthetic versions of androgens. Wait a second. Perfume is made up of all sorts of breakdown products of male sweat. Isn't perfume supposed to like smell good to guys? We have a deep abiding puzzle here. And there is an answer for it. Let me survey people first off. OK. Guys in the room, how many of you basically think that most perfumes smell kind of appealing? OK. How many of you don't? OK. Females, how many of you think your basic off-the-rack perfume smells appealing? OK. Well, that proves something. OK. Just complete it, how many of you don't? OK. The vast majority of perfumes are not purchased by men. The vast majority are purchased by women. In other words, most of the marketing decisions about what to stick in your perfumes are being marketed for people who are going to decide if it's appealing or not, people who have lots of estrogen in their bloodstream rather than androgens. That is thought to be the explanation for how it is that most perfumes are derived from male pheromones. Yeah. The other thing is if I were to wear perfume it would more so that girls think I smell good instead of for me to think I smell god. OK. Yes. The strategizing starts. OK. So a whole new world of potential neuromarketing here. But this taps into, what are the chemical constituents of pheromones? What sort of information is carried by pheromones, olfactory communication, between genders? It will tell you the species of the individual. It will tell you their gender. It will tell you whether they are gonadally intact, whether they've been castrated or not. It will tell you something about their health. It will tell you whether they are terrified or not. Have the sweat from someone or something that is terrified, and as we already know, it will smell differently to the amygdala. It will have a lot more glucocorticoid breakdown products in it. And it will tell you, as we know already, if it's the right species, if this person is related to you. Final point before we then go into the specifics of what pheromones are doing to the neurobiology of all this, finally, not only do you need to have hormones intact in order to generate the pheromones, you need to have the right reproductive hormones on board in order to perceive them. Men who are castrated no longer find the smells of female ovarian-- OK, I'm getting ahead of myself here. OK. If you don't have the hormones on board, if you have no estrogen and you're female, or if you have no testosterone and you are male, you will not be able to distinguish the sweat of women and men. Gonadally intact people can at above the chance level. Finally, women become far better at detecting the smell of-- distinguishing the smell of men versus women when they are ovulating. Finally, finally, that's not what you see when you have gay individuals. Gay men are better at detecting the smell of gay men than either straight men or straight women. So we've gotten the first pieces of here with the pheromonal system. You have to be hormonally intact to generate pheromones that are sexually informative and to detect them. What we will then transition to is, what sort of information is being carried in the pheromones and what effect does it have on the neurobiology of depending on who's pheromones you are sniffing? OK. So we will-- For more, please visit us at stanford.edu.
Info
Channel: Stanford
Views: 1,356,708
Rating: 4.8720737 out of 5
Keywords: Science, Interdisciplinary, Bioengineering, Genetic, Sociobiology, Darwin, Physiology, Evolution, Neurobiology, Sexual, Species, Natural Selection, Learning, Organism, Environment, Heritability, Reproduce, Reproduction, Survive, Gene, Variability, Mutatio
Id: LOY3QH_jOtE
Channel Id: undefined
Length: 101min 42sec (6102 seconds)
Published: Tue Feb 01 2011
Related Videos
Note
Please note that this website is currently a work in progress! Lots of interesting data and statistics to come.