HAROLD ROTH: Good
evening, everybody. We're starting on
Brown time, which is roughly seven minutes
after the announced start, just to give people
a chance to get here and to follow
existing practices. My name is Harold Roth,
and I'm the director of the Contemplative Studies
program here at Brown. And I'd like to welcome
you this evening and say a few words to you
about Contemplative Studies-- what it is that we do. I'm also curious about
where we're drawing our audience from this evening. So how many found out about
this program from our website? OK. And how many found out about
it from the medical school? No? And how many of you are
medical practitioners in one version or
another-- clinicians? That's very interesting. OK. SPEAKER 1: Get the microphone. SPEAKER 2: Yeah, you
have to unmute yourself. Unmute the podium mic. Right. HAROLD ROTH: Oh, here. SPEAKER 2: Secrets. There you go. HAROLD ROTH: Podium mic. Testing. So should I redo
what I just said? No, everybody heard me, at
least at some low level, I hope. So again, I'd like to
welcome you this evening and tell you a few
words about what we do in Contemplative Studies. We're one of the newest,
if not the newest, concentration at Brown. If you don't know what
a concentration is, it's what most
schools call a major. We're a concentration in
the exciting new field of Contemplative Studies. It's a new field that combines
scientific and humanistic approaches to studying
human contemplative experience across
cultures and across time from a number of perspectives. And we include in that-- teaching contemplative practices
in a classroom setting. So the reason that
we can do this is that unlike contemplative
practices that you would learn at a contemplative
practice center, such as a monastery, a
temple, a mosque, a synagogue, a church-- what we do
in Contemplative Studies is we teach
first-person engagement with a contemplative practice. We teach the context, the
cognitive framework in which that practice is embedded. But we do not ask, we
do not require a belief in the verticality
of that framework, and that's what
differentiates us. We study both still
meditations, sitting meditations such as mindfulness. We study moving
meditation such as qigong. And we have approaches
which include the sciences and the humanities. And the humanities
approaches help us understand the cultural
context, from which the contemplative practices
that we study in our research, have emerged. Tonight's lecture
by Dr. Judson Murray is an example of the kind of
contemplative science research that we think is extremely
important for the academic field of Contemplative Studies. I'd like to also
mention that Thursday night, between 6:00 and
8:00, we have an open house for students, or
anybody, really, from the community who's
interested in learning more about Contemplative
Studies, about the courses that we teach. We have three new courses
coming up in the second semester and also, learning about the
concentration, what it entails, what are the requirements? And to meet some of the
people who are involved, both students and
faculty members. So that's Thursday. It's just behind here
in Smith-Buonanno, 106. And it starts at
6 o'clock, and it is going to be catered by Kabob
and Curry, a constant favorite on the Brown campus. So it gives me great
pleasure tonight to introduce our speaker
Dr. Judson Brewer. Jud earned a bachelor degree
in chemistry from Princeton, and an MD in medical science
from Washington University Medical School in St.
Louis, a PhD in immunology, and did a residency
in psychiatry at Yale, spending 12 years in a
post-baccalaureate study and research,
surpassing the 10 years that the current speaker spent. Once you discover something
you love, you try to keep at it as long as you can. So he has been a
researcher at Yale and continues as an adjunct
assistant professor there. He's affiliated with MIT. He's a tenured
associate professor at UMass Medical School. He is the acting director of
the Center for Mindfulness, started by Jon
Kabat-Zinn, who founded mindfulness-based
stress reduction, and is about to be named
the head of the division of mindfulness, the
first in the country at UMass Medical School. He is the author of 37
articles, and seven chapters, and the book that he will be
signing this evening as soon as his lecture is
over on craving. And his work really focuses
on mindfulness and addiction, mindfulness-based interventions,
and the brain mechanisms of mindfulness. So it is with great
pleasure that I ask you to join me in giving
a warm welcome to Dr. Judson Brewer. [APPLAUSE] JUDSON BREWER: Thank you. Thank you for that warm
and wonderful introduction. The field of
Contemplative Studies and contemplative science is
actually a pretty young field. And what I thought
I'd start out with-- I'm sure many of you
have seen a lot of hype around mindfulness
in the modern day. So what I'd actually
like to start out with is just a little
bit of a caveat, and let this kind of color
how we talk over the next hour or so, about the mechanisms
of how our minds work and how mindfulness
actually targets these. So what I thought we could-- this is not a negative
cautionary note. This is just a cautionary
note that there can be a lot of
excitement around things. It's funny that
this is considered a new thing in the West. It's pretty old when you take
less of a Western-centric view. But needless to say, I think
one way to look at this, and really explore this for
finding what elements are really solid, is
looking in a way that I think of
as triangulation. We'll talk about that in a
minute, where we bring together theory, behavior, and
neural mechanisms. And when we find
those come together, we have this sweet
spot that really helps us understand things,
and really bring together some solidity, where there might
not be solidity in other places where there's a
lot of excitement. And importantly, replication
is really important in science. We look to repeat experiments
and with those repetitions gain more confidence in something
actually having something that's going to
last for a while. Science in general-- we see
science cycling through itself every 10 or 15 years or so. With some newer sciences,
that slows down over time as the field becomes
more established. So this is a pretty young field. And most importantly, as
an addiction psychiatrist, I like to-- it's really important to me
to see these things linked to real-world outcomes. Otherwise, we get stuck
in our ivory towers and not much help makes
it out to the real world. So just as a way to
think of triangulation, you can think of
things coming together. This kind of looks
like a bio-hazard sign. So I don't mean that-- for
it to look off-putting. But where we have
things that overlap, we look for this sweet
spot in the middle. And where that sweet spot is-- often good things happen. So the other cautionary note
that many folks are seeing here in Rhode Island-- we're
certainly seeing this in Massachusetts-- is an opioid epidemic. This is just a
heat map that shows the evolution over the last
decade and a little bit more in how the opioid
epidemic has grown. In The New York Times, they
said that deaths from overdoses are reaching levels
similar to the HIV-- the epidemic at its peak. So I say this because if we can
understand how our minds work, we can start to
develop treatments that can actually
start to reverse these things at their core. So this is opioids. Here's another map. And this just flips
back and forth between males and
females, where we can also see a growing epidemic
that's described as an obesity epidemic as well. This happened over a little
bit longer period of time. I don't know if
you can see this. This started back in the '80s
when they started mapping this. But you can see in 2015
when this came out-- a huge difference in the
levels of obesity in the US. I'm just going to
skip through this. This is just another map
showing some of the epicenters on a more fine-grained level. And you can see
in the Northeast, we're really hit pretty hard. So we can ask
ourselves, well, how far have we evolved as humans? And so I'm just going to
show you a video that's indicative of this. So this is a woman
holding her cell phone, and we can see what
happened there. So we can tongue-in-cheek
say, well, we as humans--
we're really smart. Yet in modern-day
New Haven, they've had to paint on the
crosswalks, "Look up." Because the students
for some reason are suddenly more prone
to getting into accidents, and I'm not sure that
it's because their level of intelligence is lower. I think more that
there are things that are tapping into our neural
mechanisms that are really getting in the way. And so I'll use a story of this
to kind of illustrate this. I was in Paris for the first
time in 2014, and I went to-- of course, went to The Louvre. Many of you recognize this
to do some sightseeing. And the reason I took
this picture was two-fold. One, this device
that you can see in the middle of the
picture, which we all know, was named by Time magazine
one of their top 25 inventions of the year. Some of you have heard
me joke that, to me, it's a sign of apocalypse. But the other reason
I took this picture was that the guy
in the foreground here is this boyfriend
who's now rendered obsolete. So we can imagine how
this story gets set up. We're at The Louvre. We're really excited,
and our brain says, oh, this is awesome. I want this good
feeling to continue, so it says, take a picture. And then it says, no, no, no. I have a better idea. Why don't you take a picture
and post it on Facebook? And so, how do we spend
the rest of the afternoon? Looking at the
artwork in The Louvre, or checking to see
how many likes we got? Right? So we can see how this-- our reward-based learning
systems get co-opted. And we'll talk-- we're going
to dive into that in a second. I just have to highlight
that the French-- they always do things in style. Vive la France. And so, how does
this get set up? There were some studies that
were done a couple of years ago at-- one at Harvard where they
put people in an fMRI scanner, and they gave them two options. They said, you can earn money,
or you can talk about yourself. And guess what
people chose to do? As they were
self-disclosing, they were activating the part of
the brain called the nucleus accumbens, which is the
downstream reward pathway, where dopamine gets spritzed
from the ventral tegmental area. This is the reward-based
learning system that's known to get activated
with every single drug of abuse. So whether it's alcohol,
heroin, cocaine, or Facebook, there's something that this
pathway gets activated. The second study from 2013
showed that they could actually predict the amount of time that
people spent on Facebook, based on how much their nucleus
accumbens was activated. So there's something inherently
rewarding about this system. There was a study that was
published last year where they used adolescents. They scanned adolescents
and put their own Instagram feeds in the scanner. The only manipulation
that they made was that the number of likes-- they arbitrarily manipulated
the number of likes that the pictures got. So some pictures got more
likes, and some got fewer likes. With fMRI studies, what we
look for is a relative change in brain activity. So we have a baseline and
then a comparison condition. So that more "like" condition
was really interesting. What they found was not only
was the nucleus accumbens activated, but there was
a self-referential brain region called the
posterior cingulate that was also co-activated. So what this shows
is there's this linking between
reward-based learning and self-referential processing. We're going to dive
into this a bit more as we go through the talk. So here's my interim summary
slide you can see so far. Now, it's interesting. Some are now calling this
Facebook Addiction Disorder. So if you imagine these
two women at The Louvre, they learn. Their brain learns, oh, if
I post pictures on Facebook, this feels good. And then when they're back
in their little cubicle at work and not feeling so good,
maybe it's raining outside, they've had a tough day at work. Their brain says, why
don't you go on Facebook and make yourself feel better? Which, of course,
doesn't fix whatever their root of the
problem was and leaves-- as you can see
from this diagram, leads to negative outcomes. In fact, there's a
linear correlation between the amount of social
media use and the odds-- adjust the odds ratio
of becoming depressed. You can see that on
the far end there. It's a pretty nice
linear relationship. So we can observe real changes
in our behavior, in our brains with-- I'm just using social
media as an example. And we can then start
to ask ourselves, well, mechanistically,
what's going on here? And here we actually turned
to the ancient Buddhist psychologists, because they had
a very interesting observation. In fact, this phenomenon
is said to have been described by the
historical Buddha on the night of his enlightenment. So he became enlightened
by contemplating this thing that he called
dependent origination. Now, if this is something that
can get somebody enlightened, I'm going to pay attention
to that as a scientist and say, what is this
guy talking about? So the basic idea here is
that some cure, a trigger comes into our mind. It gets interpreted as
pleasant or unpleasant. We want the pleasant
to continue. We want the unpleasant to go
away, so we act accordingly. So we have an urge, or we
have a craving for pleasant. It's often described as
aversion to the unpleasant. And then we act to keep
the pleasant going to make the unpleasant things go away. And the way they describe
this in ancient times was that this led to the
birth of a self-identity. We become identified
with our behaviors. So if I get stressed out
and that doesn't feel good, I have an urge for that
unpleasant feeling to go away. I eat some chocolate
or I eat a cupcake. Then I start to become
identified with cupcakes. I start wearing what we
describe in modern day as subjective bias
glasses, where I start seeing the world through-- if I'm stressed out, I should
eat chocolate-- glasses, OK? In ancient times, it's
really interesting. They describe this process. They use the term ignorance,
which is real interesting. We call it subjective
bias in modern day. They called it ignorance,
because they're saying, we're not seeing clearly. We're starting to see
through these lenses of our previous behaviors,
and those are coloring the way that we're seeing the world. So that's really important. The other thing that
they pointed out-- they said-- see that
loop in this pathway? They called this samsara,
which literally translated means endless wandering. And I think that's interesting,
because if we eat chocolate to feel better, we're
not actually fixing the core root of our problem. So they're saying,
well, the more we do these things,
the more we're going to endlessly
wander, and the more we're going to become identified
with these behaviors in vain, as we try to hold on to these
to make ourselves feel better. I like this quote
from Alan Watts, a modern-day philosopher. He said, the "Ego,
the self which he believes himself
to be, is nothing but a pattern of habits." And what we're
talking about here, if we just break it down into
its really simple elements-- the necessary and sufficient
components-- we need a trigger, we need a behavior,
and we need a reward. So If I'm stressed,
it's my trigger. A chocolate--
that's my behavior. And my reward is I feel
a little bit better. I'm going to show you a
Weight Watchers commercial. This is a one-minute
Weight Watchers-- [VIDEO PLAYBACK] - [SINGING] If you're
happy and you-- JUDSON BREWER: --commercial. - --know it, eat a snack. If you're happy and you
know it, eat a snack. If you're happy and you
know it, then your face will surely show it. If you're happy and you
know it, eat a snack. If you're saddened and
you know it, eat a snack. If you're sad and you
know it, eat a snack. If you're sad because
you're angry, feeling down, or chillingly bad, if
you're sad, eat a snack. If you're bored and you
know it, eat a snack. If you're lonely and you
know it, eat a snack. If you're sleepy and you
know it, if you're guilty and you know it, if you're
stressed, eat a snack. If you're human and you
know it, then your face will surely show it. If you're human, eat your
feelings, eat a snack. [END PLAYBACK] JUDSON BREWER: So I don't
own stock in Weight Watchers, but I did think that this
was a brilliant commercial. How many of you have
seen this commercial? There are very few
people, if anybody. Yeah. One of my friends sent
this to me from YouTube. And when I was talking to
some folks at Weight Watchers Corporate, I said this
is really brilliant. You've really captured
this learning process. Why don't we see
this on television? And she sheepishly said, oh,
it makes people depressed. So I guess the point system or
whatever Weight Watchers does, doesn't really target that. We'll actually talk
about that in a minute. So what we're talking
about here, whether it was the ancient Buddhist
psychologist 2,500 years ago, describing this process
with their terms dependent origination, or
modern day, some of you might have recognized,
hey, wait a minute. Isn't this positive and
negative reinforcement? Isn't this operant conditioning? Isn't this
reward-based learning? Isn't this
reinforcement learning? All of those terms are
for the same thing, and I'm just using
some examples here. You can see from the
references, this process has been described in modern
day, back in the 1800s. Thorndike did some
animal studies with this. BF Skinner became famous in
the '50s for his Skinner boxes. Eric Kandel got the
Nobel Prize in 2001, showing that this is
evolutionarily conserved all the way back to the sea slug. So a very, very evolutionarily
conserved process and well-known, well-described. So if I'm a cancer
researcher and I'm trying to cure a cancer,
what I'm going to do is look for a protein
pathway like a protein kinase pathway, so that it's mutated. I'm going to target that. And if I can target
that, I'm going to have a very
effective treatment that has minimal side effects. And what I would say is
behavioral researchers and treatment developers-- I would say we
should hold ourselves to the same standards. We should really try to
understand mechanistically how behavior works
and then target that. So here's a really
nicely described process. It's probably-- this
is not controversial. Operant conditioning has
been known for a long time. So let's use treatment
examples here. So if we understand
mechanistically what's going on, we
can develop treatment. So for example, with
alcoholism, there's this saying, people,
places, and things. Some of you might
have heard this. So if we can avoid cues-- if we can help people who
drink avoid cues, like avoid the bars, their drinking
buddies, the liquor store, they're less likely to
be triggered to drink. It makes sense mechanistically. That's a little harder to
do with things like smoking where people learn to
smoke on their front porch, in their car, outside at work. So there's this development
of substitute strategies. So eat some carrot sticks,
eat some candy, go for a walk. And we can see how this also
can target this mechanism. And interestingly,
neither of these actually targets the
core loop itself. You can see those orange
arrows in there are not-- this doesn't get at
those orange arrows. It can help us
stop feeding them, or we can treat
around the behaviors. But it doesn't actually
dismantle the loop at its core. So if we go back to the
ancient Buddhist psychologists and say, well, what can we do? They actually gave some
very interesting pointers. He said, "Just as a
tree, though cut down, can grow again and
again if its roots are undamaged and strong,
in the same way, if the roots of craving
are not wholly uprooted, sorrows will come
again and again." That's interesting. If you don't quite make--
can't make sense of that, here's a modern-day
interpretation, but we get the idea. So they're actually
talking about craving, and not substituting strategies,
or avoiding cues, but actually, getting rid of the
heart of craving. So again, if we
understand mechanistically how something works, it can
be very powerful information for us. So just-- for example, the food
industry knows this very well. For those of you that haven't
seen this expose in The New York Times from a
couple of years ago, they talk very critically about
and importantly about the food engineers that are employed to
make things that look and taste like food. And I say that because
I love The Onion, the satirical journal. They had this
headline that said, "Doritos Celebrates its One
Millionth Ingredient," right? So we can engineer things that
have the perfect smell, taste, color, crunch, et cetera, et
cetera, to get us addicted. Here's the summary slide for
a Cookie Monster, so just an example of how powerful
these processes are. And if we understand
how they work, how we can manipulate them-- so why am I-- I am not trying to give
a talk that's depressing or a downer for us. But I'm just pointing
out how important it is to really
understand things from a mechanistic perspective. And if we can understand
those, then we can start to affect a
real process change, OK? So many of you are familiar-- I work at the Center for
Mindfulness, jokingly called the house that Jon built. Because Jon Kabat-Zinn
really helped jump-start this Western
movement of mindfulness, and he's really well-known for
this working definition of what mindfulness actually is. And he actually has
modified it a bit. He talks about
the awareness that arises from "paying attention in
the present moment on purpose, non-judgmentally." Now, for the uninitiated,
this can seem like a mouthful. So if we bring this
back to the mechanisms-- those positive and negative
reinforcement mechanisms-- and then really highlight
that craving piece, there's awareness
that can arise. And then we can be sucked in, or
we can be pushing things away. What mindfulness is
about is this awareness that has a balance or
equanimity with it, where we're not sucked in. We're not rejecting
what's happening. We're simply being
with something in a way that I like to use the
term like curiosity, or that really helps
unpack what we mean by non-judgemental awareness. So again, we can start to
bring triangulation together, around what mindfulness
is, for example. And so we can-- what we like
to do is really understand the theory of how
these practices work, bring this together with
behavioral and neural mechanisms, and
then, importantly, see how this affects
real behavior change. And that's what I'm going to go
through some examples of now. So bring this back
to the theory. Here's a mechanism. The theory is that mindfulness
comes in and drives this wedge of awareness in,
such that we have space. Instead of habitually
reacting, we can take a pause, and mindfully, or with
awareness choose a behavior rather than habitually reacting. But this might seem like that's
a tall order with regard-- can we just pay attention? Will that actually help
us change behavior? It might seem
somewhat paradoxical. So I'll give you an example
from one of our first smoking studies, where we
randomize people to get mindfulness training
or gold standard treatment. We didn't even
tell them that they were getting mindfulness
training when they came in, because we didn't
want to bias them. And on the first
day of training, we told them to smoke. And they looked at us like,
is this the experiment that you guys are doing? Because I came here
to quit smoking. You're telling me to smoke. But importantly,
we're telling them, OK, go ahead and smoke,
but pay attention as you do and see what happens. And here's an example of what
people typically wake up to. This person said it
"smells like stinky cheese and tastes like chemicals. Yuck!" So the critical
piece here is not that they're trying to convince
themselves that smoking is bad. They all came into our program
trying to quit smoking. What we're having
them do is really tap into reward-based learning. So if reward-based
learning is so powerful, can we actually hack it? Can we really help people see
the results or the rewards of their behavior? Reward is in quotes, because
those rewards might not be as rewarding if we
pay attention to them, just like this
person pointed out. So what we're-- reward-based
learning isn't actually driven by behavior, which
seems kind of odd. We always think of,
oh, if I could just change my behavior, if I
could just change my behavior. Well, the truth is that
behavior is driven by reward. So what if we not necessarily
change the reward, but help our brains
see those rewards really, really clearly, OK? Or as Yogi Berra put
it, "You can observe a lot just by watching," right? So how well does this
stuff actually work? I'm not going to go
through the details, because we've published these
studies a few years ago. But just to show you,
in our smoking study, we got five times the quit rates
of gold standard treatment. We were actually
looking for a signal. This was a pretty strong signal. We weren't expecting
it to be this strong. So for us, that said, wow,
that's really interesting. Mindfulness-- we can see
behavioral change, OK? And so if we can see
behavioral change, we can then go back, and start
looking at it mechanistically, and say, what's going on here? And our hypothesis was that
mindfulness would decouple craving and the behavior. So for example, smoking-- I used the analogy. And actually, this is an
analogy found all the way back in the early teachings. The idea is that we should see
a dissociation between craving and smoking before
they both subside. So if you think of craving
as a fire and that fire is burning every
time you add fuel to it, every time you
smoke a cigarette, you're adding fuel to that fire. When you stop adding
fuel to that fire, that fire continues to burn,
until the residual fuel is consumed. And we can actually test
this mathematically. So we should still see
craving, but that should be decoupled from smoking itself. So when we look at baseline,
we can see a strong correlation between craving
and smoking, just like other people have found. If you crave, you smoke. If you crave, you smoke. At the end of
treatment, we found that that correlation was
completely abolished-- was completely gone. And when we looked and did all
the fancy math around that, we found that it had nothing
to do with baseline craving or cigarette use,
but it had everything to do with informal
mindfulness practices. And these were formally
moderating the decoupling of craving and behavior. So if you bring it back to this
mechanistic loop, as theorized, it does seem to be driving this
wedge in where people can act. They can have a craving
without acting on that craving, I should say. And so in this sense,
it may actually affect longer-lasting change. So again, bringing this back
to the triangulation question, it does seem like the
theory is lining up with our behavioral
mechanisms and also, lining up with
behavioral outcomes. So for example, these five times
quit rates that we were seeing. Now, as we were developing
this first program, I was looking out my window. We were on a smoke-free
campus, and I was looking at my patients. And they were out in
the parking lot smoking. And so they had a
cigarette in one hand. And what did they have
in their other hand? Oh, actually, sorry. I meant to go through
this, so I'm going to skip. That slide is
slightly out of place. So this is what I meant to
say-- was what we were seeing was people in the parking lot. They were distracting
themselves with their phones as they were smoking. And so we said, well, can we-- if we understand how
this thing works, the system is actually set up. So we would remember
where food is. Operant conditioning is
reward-based learning. If we're looking
for food sources and we find a food
source, we're going to lay down a memory that
says remember what you ate and where you found it. So if people are learning
through context, event, and memory, they don't actually
learn to smoke in my office. So if I'm trying to teach them
to quit smoking in my office, I'm already adding one level
of artificiality there. So what if I took my office
and delivered it to them? And this is where
we started trying to develop
evidence-based training and deliver it
through a cell phone. So I'll show you some examples. We now have three
that we're testing-- one for smoking, one
for emotional eating, and now, one even for
unwinding anxiety. And I'll walk you through
some of the clinical trials that we're doing. The basic premise here-- and I'm just using an example
from our anxiety program-- is that we can take
these trainings and deliver them in
bite-sized pieces, so people can get short
trainings in the moment. And they can go back to
those trainings each day, so they can make sure
they reinforce them. We can directly
link, helping people understand how their mind works
and link that to mindfulness. We can also have these
modules delivered in a way that are accessible. It's really hard to get
somebody to come into my office once a week, twice a week. Forget about it. And for an hour a week
is already challenging. So can we cut this
into bite-sized pieces? Can we give people in
the moment animations that can help drive
the key points home? I'll show an example of this. And then, can we give them
in-the-moment exercises? So they can-- in the
moment that they're craving for a cigarette, or a
cupcake, or they're caught-- wound up in anxiety,
can we give them a practice that they can use? And most importantly, can we
embed experience sampling, so we can track progress
and test efficacy? So I'll just show you an
example from our Craving to Quit program on how
we can use animations to drive key points home. [VIDEO PLAYBACK] - Today we're going
to explain cravings through the metaphoric
screaming child. I know you've heard and seen
a child throwing a tantrum. Perhaps for some of you,
it was your own child. This child is screaming
because he wants something. And you as the parent want
him to stop screaming. All your child
wants is a lollipop. So in this metaphor,
think of your cravings as the child's tantrum. The lollipop is
like the cigarettes. But what should you do? Yell at the child? That might make things worse. Gag the child? That will stop the
screaming in the short-term, but you know what will happen as
soon as you take the gag away. You could just give
him a lollipop. Congratulations. You just taught your child
to scream for lollipops. In other words,
you've just taught him that screaming is a good
way to get what he wants. What will you do instead? Here's a novel concept. Just let him scream himself out. What? Have you ever seen a
child scream forever? Sometimes they scream
so hard and so long that you think their head
or your head might explode, but it never does. What happens instead? Eventually, the child
gets tired and stops. [END PLAYBACK] JUDSON BREWER: So
just an example of how we can drive
these key points home in using simple analogies. So as we were
developing this program, we were finding that people were
reporting that they're actually changing their eating behavior,
which was pretty interesting. Because when people
try to quit smoking, they tend to gain weight,
on average 15 pounds. And in fact, this is
one of the key reasons that women in particular
won't quit smoking-- is that fear of weight gain. So I don't know if anybody
can relate to this. But we said, OK, can
we actually apply this to stress and emotional eating? I'll give you an example from
somebody I saw in my clinic. This is a woman. And she was-- who was at
a very unhealthy weight and come in to me. She met Binge Eating
Disorder criteria. She was binging on entire
large pizzas 20 out of 30 days. And what she reported was that
her mom had been emotionally abusing her since she
was eight, and that she was eating in order to numb
herself from her feelings. So we developed this program. It's delivered in a similar
way to the smoking cessation program, to help people change
their relationship to eating, so they could pay attention. And just like paying attention,
when you smoke a cigarette and see that it doesn't
taste very good, they could pay
attention and see what it's like when they
eat a bunch of food at once, because
that is something. And instead of numbing
themselves out, actually get curious about
what that actually feels like. And then we start to look. So clinically, if we really
want to see something stick, we want to see behavior
change language that they are reporting on their own. They're not just mirroring or
puppeting things back to us, but they're actually describing
this stuff in their own terms. So what we could do is
look at people's journals from our online
community and see what they're actually
reporting with regard to their relationship to eating. So here's an example. This person said, "I
understand why I go to food-- to avoid, or cover
up, or distract from uncomfortable
feelings such as anger, sadness, or restlessness. Who wants to feel those things? Trigger-- uncomfortable feeling. Behavior-- eat something
that temporarily diminishes the feeling. Reward-- still have to deal
with the unpleasant feelings, plus the sugar headache. I can clearly see how I got
caught in this habit loop, trying to escape difficult
feelings with food, but that ultimately,
it doesn't work." So here is somebody
describing this habit loop, understanding this, and then
starting to get disenchanted herself. And that disenchantment
piece comes when we tap into
these reward-based learning systems themselves,
rather than using some cognitive strategy, or
trying to force ourselves to change our behavior. So how well does this work? So we've got some theory. What are the behavioral results? So just a couple of weeks ago,
actually, one of my colleagues, or collaborators at
UCSF just finished up a study that was published. And the idea was
to see if we could see the same type
of behavior change with eating as we
could see with smoking. Here we found a 40% reduction--
craving-related eating, using experience sampling and
about a 36% reduction in eating to cope with
negative emotions. So we're starting to see not
only behavioral outcomes, but we're starting to also see
those line up with mechanism as well. So we see this in smoking. We see this in eating,
which is really nice. If we're seeing a triangulation
with different types of behaviors, but the
same type of training, that gives us a
lot of confidence that it might be working. But again, most
importantly, are they talking about this stuff
in their own lives? So here's another example
from somebody's journal. This person said, "A
shift is happening. I'm choosing more healthy foods. The sugary things
are less attractive. Satiety is now
coming into focus." So reward-based learning-- it's
a really powerful mechanism, and something that we
can start to tap into. And I just want to highlight
a collaborative piece that-- and actually, a couple
of folks in the room were involved with this. So we can actually start
to take that behavior, look to see where
that's also described in the ancient
literature, and develop modern-day
questionnaires that we can use to start to personalize
development, and deliver treatment in a targeted way. So for example, there's this
fifth-century commentary where they actually describe
three different types of behaviors. And roughly, they line up with
fight, flight, and freeze, which is what
we're talking about with operant conditioning, so
approach, avoid, or freeze. And they described--
this is really interesting in this commentary. They described posture. They described how people ate,
the types of food they ate, the types of clothes they wore. And this was actually done in-- we'd spent a week in developing
these questions right here, right off the Brown
campus and then validated the questions in a
large cohort of folks, where it came down
to 13 questions that we can actually
start to use to match someone's personality,
or behavioral tendency with mindfulness practices. So Jared Lindahl,
Willoughby Britton. I think Yuan was-- you were not involved
in this project. He was always around, so
I associate you with this as well. But there were some folks
within this very campus who led this project, and we
came up with something very interesting. So the idea here is that
if we understand things mechanistically, it gives us a
lot of power to affect change. So what I'd like to
do, just for the last-- maybe 15 minutes or so, is
talk about neural mechanisms. What's actually
going on in the brain as we start to change behavior
with mindfulness practices? So I'll use a story. Some of you have heard me
talk about this before. This woman is Lolo
Jones, Olympic hurdler, favored to win
the 2008 Olympics, and in fact, was in
the lead in the finals at the 9th and 10th
hurdles, and then said in an interview
with Time magazine-- she said, I was just
in an amazing rhythm. And I knew at one point
I was winning the race. It wasn't like, oh, I'm
winning the Olympic gold medal. It just seemed
like another race. And then there was
a point after that where I was telling
myself to make sure my legs were snapping
out, so I overtried. That's when I hit the hurdle. So the point here
is not-- this is one of the misconceptions
of mindfulness-- is it's not about ridding
our minds of thoughts or of thinking. It's about changing our
relationship to them, so having those thoughts
come up as objects-- being aware of them and not
being pushed or pulled by them. A simple way to think of this
is, "Your me is in the way." And we can even think of this
as a behavioral continuum as being caught up in our
relationship to objects. So daydreaming, for example. You can get caught
up in daydreaming, and then we can snap
out of that quickly. If we get caught
up in stress, it'd be great if we could just
say, hey, you look stressed. Snap out of it. Oh, thank you. Boy, that's much better. So we can imagine when
we're caught up in stress. It's a little harder
to disengage from that. And of course, on the
far end of the spectrum-- addiction. I like the simple
definition-- continued use, despite adverse consequences. So when we're so caught
up in this behavior, we know we're doing it, and
we have no control over it. That is the epitome
of addiction. And if this is
happening all the time, we can actually map this
on a neurobiological level. And this is where a
network of brain regions was discovered,
actually back in 2001, called the default mode network. This was serendipitously
discovered by Marc Raichle's group at Washington University
in St. Louis, where they were actually doing a baseline task. So again, with neuroimaging,
we have a baseline and a comparison condition. They had given people
the simple task-- lay still and don't do
anything in particular. And they figured,
well, it's not going to show anything in particular. And in fact, they were
getting reproducible results that ended up being the
most reproducible results in all of neuroimaging,
where they found this network of
brain regions constantly co-activating. And what they found,
eventually, was when we're not doing anything
in particular, we're actually doing something, on average
pretty often, which is thinking about ourselves, right? So this was the self-referential
brain region network that was getting activated. And you can see these
two yellow circles here-- the medial prefrontal cortex and
the posterior cingulate cortex. So just to remind you, the
posterior cingulate cortex is that self-referential brain
region that gets activated when we are seeing pictures,
that we get a bunch-- are pictures that
get a bunch of likes. Now, this brain
region is interesting, because it also
gets activated when you show people,
who are addicted to cocaine, pictures of
people smoking crack cocaine. It also gets activated-- it
lights up like a Christmas tree in people who are addicted to
cigarettes, when you show them pictures of people smoking. In fact, you can
see this activated in people who are pathological
gamblers, when you show them pictures of gambling scenes. And in one of my
favorite studies, as done by a friend who's
on faculty at Yale now, she did this as her PhD
thesis where she put people in a PET scanner. And in PET scanners,
you can actually feed people and still
get good imaging results. So she gave people their
favorite chocolate, and she fed people chocolate. And she had them rate how well
they liked that chocolate, and you can see
this rating scale. It went from really delicious-- plus 10 to-- this
is off at minus 10. What she did was fed
people chocolate. She says, how's that taste? And they're like, this is great. That's why I picked
this chocolate. This is awesome. That's a 10. And then she'd feed them another
piece and say, how is it now? It's still pretty good. Feed them another piece. How is it now? Well, I still like it. How is it now? All right. I think I've had enough. How is it now? How long is this study going? Why did I sign up for this? And she'd feed them until
that very piece of chocolate that made people
ecstatic made them sick. They were like, stop feeding me. I'm going to call the
IRB and report abuse. But the interesting
thing here was there was only one
brain region that got activated on both
ends of the spectrum, so when people
loved the chocolate and when they hated
the chocolate. And you can see here was the
posterior cingulate cortex. So this brain region
gets activated when people are loving it
and when they're hating it. And in fact, we can
see this across a bunch of different studies. I'm just going to
show you a list of a bunch of
different studies that show activation in the
posterior cingulate cortex. Not that you have to
memorize this list. The idea here is just to see
all the different things that can activate the posterior
cingulate cortex. So we started asking
the question, well, what's actually going on? How does this line up with
somebody's experience? And we came up with
this hypothesis that the posterior cingulate was
involved in getting caught up in our experience. So Lolo Jones who got caught
up in her own thinking-- when we get caught
up in craving, we get caught up in
loving chocolate. When we get caught up
in hating chocolate, this brain region might be
involved, and we can test that. So just to bring this into
an experiential level, there was a study that was
done a couple of years ago, and actually induced a bunch of
emotions in people, and said, where do you feel those
emotions most strongly? So for example-- we can
all do this together here. If you remember a time
when you were really afraid and you can just-- if you care to,
just take a moment to feel what it feels like where
you feel fear in your body, and you can mentally
point it at-- and then I'll show
you how you line up with the study of
800 people when they did the same type of induction. So if this was the case
for you, where you felt it in your chest, that's
pretty congruent with what this population sample
showed and in fact, across a number of emotions. So for example,
fear and anxiety-- we can see this hot
spot right in the chest. So we can start to
feel where we feel it when we get caught up in
emotions, like fear, emotions like anxiety. So if we get caught
up in things, what's the task of
mindfulness training? Well, you can think of it as
a way to not get caught up, not to get caught
up in fear, not to get caught up in anxiety,
or anger, excitement, or caught up in anything. And again, here we can start
to bring in our triangulation. So what we did was said, well,
if we took three different type of meditation practices,
can we triangulate where we see those practices
activating in the brain? And we might be able to
find areas of commonality. So again, lining up the
theory with the mechanism and the behavior. So what we found-- actually, we didn't find
a single brain region that was increased in activity. But we found,
shown here in blue, was these two main hubs of
the default mode network were deactivated. They were quiet-- inexperienced
versus novice meditators. So this was really interesting. It wasn't what we
were expecting, but it started to make sense. With meditation it's not
about getting caught up in our experience. It's about letting go
of that experience. And we're seeing, if the
posterior cingulate lines up with getting caught up,
we're seeing it getting quiet with these practices, regardless
of what they are that help us let go of it. And as I mentioned
at the beginning, replication is really important. So these were some
of the first studies that had kind of
shown this, and so we wanted to look to see if
this was replicatable. We did a replication study. I won't show you the
details, because it's not that important. And there was a
recent meta-analysis that showed not
many brain regions that are convergent across a
bunch of different studies. But you can see here
the posterior cingulate is one of those that
actually lines up. And when we look specifically
at loving kindness practice, for example, where we're
offering well-wishing to ourselves and
others, you can see in particular the posterior
cingulate gets really quiet. Again, blue is deactivation. So importantly,
replication is helpful, but we wanted to take
this one step further. And one of my colleagues at Yale
had developed a methodology, where he could actually
deliver feedback from somebody's
brain in real time, as a way to help us really
line up subjective experience directly with brain activity. And so what we can do
is we can have somebody meditate where we
can study their-- we can measure
their brain activity and actually present it to them,
basically with a short delay, close to real time. We can do a bunch of
control experiments to make sure that we're
not fooling ourselves, and they're not
fooling themselves. And the way this works
is that we have somebody lay on the scanner. They're meditating
with their eyes open. And what they do is check
in with the feedback graph every couple of seconds and
see how well that graph, as it fills in, would
represent increased and decreased activity in the
posterior cingulate cortex-- how well that lines up
with their brain activity. And you can see a
couple of runs here. You see a lot of variability. Variability is actually
helpful for us, so we can see-- line that up with
subjective experience. We can ask people, how well does
that increase in activity line up with your subjective
experience of mind wandering, or getting caught
up in your experience? And how well do the
decreases correlate with you being on task? In this case, we're using breath
awareness meditation, I think. Here you can see a novice's
very strong correspondence between subjective
experience in brain activity. You can see the same
reported correspondence in experienced
meditators, but you can see how these brains
are pretty different. Just to give you an
example of the really rich phenomenological self-reports
that we can derive from this, here's an example of somebody
who-- they report directly after each run. This person said,
"I caught myself. I was trying to guess when
the words were going to end--" that was our baseline task-- "and when the meditation
was going to begin. So it's kind of
like, ready, set, go. And there's this additional
word that popped up, and I was like, oh, shit,
and there's a red spike. And then I was
sort of settled in. I was really getting into it. And then I thought, oh,
my gosh, this is amazing. It's describing exactly
what I'm saying, and then you see this red spike. And then I was like, OK,
don't get distracted. And I got into it, and
it went blue again. I was like, oh, my gosh,
this is unbelievable. It's doing exactly
what my mind is doing." So he was laughing
at this point. He said, to the next
point, it's a perfect map of what my mind
is going through. So here in a single
subject, they can say this maps perfectly
with my experience. But for us as
scientists, we want to say, well, what exactly
is this mapping on to? And so here is
where we do things like torture undergraduates
and have them actually transcribe all of the rich
phenomenological descriptions and in a blinded manner, line
them up with brain activity. So I'm going to have to give a
shout-out to Juan Santoyo who did all of this work as an
undergrad here at Brown. And we did this work
in collaboration with the late Cathy Kerr, who
is an amazing person in general, and also an amazing
collaborator to work with. So we were very fortunate to
work with her on this study. And the way this works-- you can see all of
the different ways that people describe
their direct experience. And these are
coded and described as open codes, which
then move or link together in central codes
and then theoretical codes. And then we can line these up
with brain activity, which gets really, really interesting. So for example, when people
reported a fair amount of distracted awareness, this
replicates a lot of other work that people had done, showing
that the posterior cingulate cortex gets activated
when we're mind wandering, when we're getting distracted. So we could think of this
as a positive control for this experiment. But in fact, we found this
whole other side of things that we named controlling. So when people were efforting
or when they were discontent, it was also activating the
posterior cingulate cortex, which wasn't in the
literature, so that was really interesting
to us-- a new discovery. And here's an example. This person said, "I worried
that I wasn't using the graph as an object of meditation. So I tried to look at it harder
or somehow pay attention more," because when it went red. So this trying--
it's like you're hearing the sound of my
voice during this lecture. Trying to hear it
harder doesn't really make sense, because
your ears know how to hear as long as you
don't have hearing problems. So the trying piece
is actually optional, and this person discovered this. Here's some other examples
of deactivation lining up with brain activity-- I'm sorry, with
subjective experience. So again, undistracted
awareness. We saw lots of instances
of concentration. This, again, replicated
previous literature when people were on task. Their posterior cingulate
gets quiet, so nice, positive control. Again, another category
emerged that was kind of the opposite
of the effort, which Yuan named effortless doing. So when we're not trying to do
things-- when we're content, we're also seeing a deactivation
of the posterior cingulate cortex. Here's some examples. This person said,
"Toward the middle, I had some thoughts, which
I don't see in the graph, maybe because I let
them kind of flow by. I noticed the more I relaxed and
stopped trying to do anything, the bluer it went." So great, now I understand what
Yoda was talking about, right? So it's about--
these practices can be challenging in terms
of trying to meditate and in fact, what the
theory and what we're seeing with this brain
activity is suggesting-- it's not about trying. It's about setting
up the conditions. So we might be
asking, OK, great. You guys studied a bunch of
very experienced meditators. How does this actually
apply to me or anybody else in real life? These folks on average
have been practicing, just coincidentally,
about 10,000 hours. We weren't trying to
surpass that Malcolm Gladwell's popularized
10,000-Hour Rule, which may not have a ton
of great science behind it. But these folks happened to be
practicing about 10,000 hours. So we could be asking,
does it really take time? I'd better start
meditating right now. Excuse me if I leave the
lecture hall, because I'm going to start meditating
for the rest of my life to try to catch up. So here I think
it's really helpful to say, well, what are these
data actually showing us? And this is where-- I'm sorry to use a sports
person to describe this, but Vince Lombardi--
he was the guy that they gave the Super Bowl
trophy out-- the Lombardi Trophy. He was the guy that won
the first Super Bowl-- Green Bay Packers coach. He talks about perfect
practice, not just practicing. And so just as a
personal example, I grew up playing the violin. And if I practiced my
scales out of tune, it was actually worse than
not practicing at all. Because I would
learn incorrectly as compared to
practicing correctly. So what is this
perfect practice about? What is it about mindfulness? And we can think of this
as bringing ingredients together as compared to forcing
ourselves to be mindful. So we hear this thing--
paying attention, so pay attention, pay attention. Here's a novice meditator. So these are three-minute runs. So just learn to meditate. We put him in the scanner. We give him the feedback. And his job is just to line
up his subjective experience with his brain activity. This person said, "I
feel a lot more relaxed, like it was less of
a struggle to prevent my mind from wandering." After three runs,
his brain activity looked completely different. We didn't give him
a brain transplant. He just was actually
using this as feedback, even though that wasn't
part of the experiment. What he was describing-- if you
go back to the ancient theory, it is what's described as the
fifth factor of awakening. Passaddhi-- I'm
not a Pali scholar, so from what I understand,
this is translated as relaxation or tranquility. And what this person
discovered was if he stopped trying to force
himself to pay attention to his breath,
his brain activity was completely different. It was actually easier
for him to pay attention. Here's an example of an
experienced meditator who described focusing on the
breath and in particular, the feeling of interest,
wonder, and joy that arise in conjunction
with mindful breathing. Look at this brain activity. So here-- what this
person is describing is one of the second
factors of awakening, or the second factor. There's only one. Interest or curiosity. And even that joy-- it can roughly line up
with another factor-- the fourth factor of awakening
where there's joy that arises. And we can even
start to line this up with reward-based
learning, because joy itself can be rewarding. Here's another
novice meditator who said, I don't think
your feedback works, because it was red, and I
was thinking about my breath. Well, look at the very next run. Look how different his brain is. And he said, oh, I get it. Feeling the physical sensation,
instead of thinking in and out. So here-- think of
this as dropping the self, getting
out of our own way. So you can see
here some examples of people learning to change
their brain activity in as few as nine minutes. So the graduate student I was
working with at that time said, why don't we actually
translate this to something more pragmatic? Because fMRI-- it
costs about $1,000 to run anybody through
our scanner for one of these paradigms,
so that's not going to be
clinically pragmatic. He said, well, what about EEG? So we just published
our first paper, showing that we can
actually do similar things. It might not be quite
as good spatially, but the temporal--
the time resolution is even much better
with EEG neurofeedback where we might be able to start
moving this into the clinics. We just got a grant a little
while ago from the Fetzer Trust to try to develop something
that will actually move into the clinics
a little more readily. And in fact, a
couple of years ago, we had somebody come into
the lab to try this out. Some of you might recognize him. I'll just show you
the short video. [VIDEO PLAYBACK] - This is just the next
generation's exercise. We've got the physical
exercise components down. And now, it's about
working out, how can we actually train our minds? - Dr. Brewer is
trying to understand how mindfulness can alter
the functioning of the brain. He uses a cap lined
with 128 electrodes. - We're going to start filling
each of these 128 wells with conduction gel. - The electrodes are
able to pick up signals from the posterior cingulate,
part of a brain network linked to memory and emotion. - This is all just picking
up electrical signal from the top of your head. - Since attending the
Mindfulness Retreat, I've been meditating daily. I was curious to see if it
had an impact on my brain. - We're going to have you start
with thinking of something that was very
anxiety-provoking for you. - When I thought about
something stressful, the cells in my brain's
posterior cingulate immediately started
firing, shown by the red lines that went
off the chart on the computer screen. - Just drop into meditation. - When I let go of
those stressful thoughts and refocused on my
breath, within seconds the brain cells
that had been firing quieted down, shown by the
blue lines on the computer. - That's really fascinating
to see them like that. - Dr. Brewer believes everyone
can train their brains to reach that blue mindfulness zone. And he says all the
technology we're surrounded by makes it difficult. [END PLAYBACK] JUDSON BREWER: So here-- just an example. I think where we can start to
now bring in neural mechanisms, and even have that synergized
with what we understand, about how the mind works
with reward-based learning. And here I think it's
like-- this obviously doesn't take the place
of a meditation teacher, or learning how to meditate. But we can provide these
mental mirrors, perhaps, that can help people
see very clearly, and link up their subjective
experience with their brain activity, and learn from that. And here I think if
we bring all of this back together in triangulating
the theory with the mechanisms with the behavior, we
can even say, well, how do we actually use
these practices to hack this very reward-based learning
system that is the strongest one that's known? So for example, if the
typical behavior is-- let's use stress or
anxiety as a trigger. The behavior is whatever we
do to distract ourselves, whether it's this behavior, or
eating something, or smoking a cigarette, or
drinking, or whatever we do to make ourselves
feel temporarily better. And that reward is
that temporary relief. We can start to look at these
and say, well, wait a minute. These are externally-driven
behaviors. I need my smartphone. I need that cupcake. I need that cigarette. And I get a temporary relief. What if we provided a
substitute strategy, something that is always available
that's intrinsic? So if that trigger
is anxiety or stress, what if we got curious about
what that felt like in our body in that moment? Suddenly, we've got
an intrinsic behavior that's always available. And in fact, the reward
is pretty different. So instead of that excited
reward of eating that cupcake, we start to tap into things
like curiosity and joy which feel different. Experimentally there's more of
an expansive quality to that as compared to the
contractive quality of some dopaminergic reward. Now, importantly, we look
for behavior change language, to see if this is
actually true when people are going through this program,
for these different types of programs. So I'll just end with
a couple examples to see if this is
actually true as we start to collect more and more data. So this is somebody
from our smoking program that said, "I'm able
to somehow ride out the craving pretty quickly. I think of it as a new
kind of habit loop. I go from wanting a smoke to
either automatically recalling the bad sensations, or
automatically connecting the cigarette with more
fuel for my addiction," so tapping into
that negative piece of the reward-based
learning, seeing clearly what those results are. Here's an example from
our anxiety program. This person said--
somebody who's riding out full-blown panic
attacks, which obviously, not expecting. "When I had a full-blown
panic attack, looking inside made it just melt away." So they were drawing
their awareness to the physical
sensations of panic-- said, "I was looking
at what I was feeling, instead of obsessing over
why I was feeling it." This obsession piece
is really important, because with anxiety
the thinking behavior-- at some point,
planning is helpful. And then our brain
starts to learn, oh, I can think my way through this. But in fact, worried
behavior just compounds the anxiety as they
go over this event horizon, I think of as into that
black hole of anxiety. So instead of obsessing,
they were just dropping into the
physical sensations and then riding out
full-blown panic attacks. Here's an interesting one. This person said, "What's
most interesting to me is how we define the rewards. In the past, the
reward of eating right had been weight loss. But it was more often
than not short-lived, because I hadn't
made real process changes in my daily life. Here it feels like the reward
is defined differently, and weight loss
is a side effect. The reward here is, for lack
of a better expression, a more balanced life or inner peace." So they're tapping into this
balance, this peace that comes with bringing awareness
to whatever is with us and finding that that's
actually intrinsically-- perhaps even more
motivating, more driving. A better reward, so to speak,
than these externally-driven rewards such as weight loss. So just to bring it back
to the story of my patient and to end here, this person
went through our eating program and came back to me a
couple of months ago. She had lost 40 pounds, but
more importantly, she said, I feel like I've
got my life back. I can eat a single
piece of pizza now and actually enjoy it. So I will end there. And hopefully, there's
even a pragmatic element that we can all take
home from this-- is just starting to
simply see-- become aware of whatever our own habit
loops are and see what happens when we actually bring
a curious awareness to those things in the moment. We don't need 10,000
hours of practice to see what it feels
like to be caught up in some habitual
reaction versus-- be curious about that and what
that feels like in our bodies. All right. And I'll stop here
and say, I get to stand up here and
talk about this stuff. Because there are a bunch of
people that put a bunch of work into this and
importantly, the folks that funded this, including--
most of our funding is from the NIH as
well as I mentioned the Fetzer Trust and the
Mind and Life Institute. And I'd actually like to just
end by saying, yet again-- and actually, dedicate this in
the memory of Cathy Kerr, who is just such a wonderful person
for any of you that knew her here, and such a beacon
in terms of curiosity, generosity, and kindness. So I will stop
there, and I would be happy to take questions. Thank you. [APPLAUSE] I think this is recorded,
so they're going to ask you. If you have a
question-- probably to go to one of the mics. Is that right? SPEAKER 3: Yes. JUDSON BREWER: So I saw a couple
of folks with their hands up. If you could just
go to the mics, then I won't get in trouble. SPEAKER 4: Make sure you push
the button at the base of it for a few minutes, and
then I'll turn it back on. JUDSON BREWER: That
sounds like a plan. Please. AUDIENCE: I was wondering
what you thought about the application of the
neuroscience of mindfulness to athletic performance. I also wanted to
ask if you've heard about such technologies
as the FocusBand or The Fluid Motion Factor
Program, things like that. JUDSON BREWER: Yeah,
great question. So applying mindfulness
to athletic performance. Certainly, it's
popular, like the NFL. There are a number of teams now
that have mindfulness coaches. I think one of the
first was the Seahawks. Michael Gervais-- he's a
well-known mindfulness coach there. The Indianapolis Colts. Some of the other
teams now have those. So certainly, speaking
from that perspective, the NBA is actually
way ahead of the NFL. George Mumford, who
actually is on our board at the Center for
Mindfulness, had been coaching-- had been teaching. Let's see, it started
with the Bulls. They won three
championships in a row. It helped Phil Jackson
move to the Lakers. They won three
championships in a row-- could be coincidence. Who knows? So I think we certainly
see examples of that in high-performance athletics. And in fact, one of the most
well-described or highly sought after states in sports
is flow or being in the zone. Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi
described this back in the '70s,
talked about sports. And there are folks
that write popular books about these types of things,
like Steven Kotler has written some stuff around flow. So I think the concepts
line up pretty well. We actually had-- I
didn't show it here, but we had an
experienced meditator get into flow in our
scanner and report that, and we actually watched. We were doing a real-time
scan, so we watched her brain activity in the PCC plummet. And I wrote it. It ended up being a
chapter in my book. So I think there are pieces
here that line up beautifully. And Csikszentmihalyi actually
talks about meditation as a way to train flow. So if you think of-- like we get contracted
around fear. So let's use sports,
like performance anxiety. And they have names for
this in all the sports. So in golf, they
call it the yips. And they-- freezing-- it's
analysis by paralysis. All this stuff that they
talk about in pro sports. So if you think of
getting caught up in our experience, or
Lolo Jones as an example, getting caught up in
trying to make herself-- make sure her legs
are snapping out-- if you take the
opposite of that-- if you just take that
expansion to infinity-- if this is telling
us, this is who I am and the rest of the
world is outside of me, you expand to infinity. And now, you lose
the difference. You lose that boundary
between self and other. And so then we move into
selfless experiences and flow. So I think meditation lines
up beautifully with that. I wouldn't recommend any of the
commercially available things right now. I'm not sure that
the science is quite caught up with the popular
appeal and the willingness to pay a ton of money to
improve one's performance. So I don't know. I haven't seen a lot
of great science. And we've tested a lot of
these machines in our lab and haven't found
good stuff there. So I would say, be cautious with
the things that are out there. And hopefully,
down the road there will be things that are a little
more scientifically based. So hopefully, that
gets at your question. It's a really interesting and
a growing field of interest in terms of research. It's hard. Sometimes the commercialization
outpaces the research. That's where we are, Josh. Thank you. Please. AUDIENCE: I just want to say
this is absolutely tremendous and learning a lot, really
trying to absorb it. But this is kind of just a
down-to-earth observation of my years on this planet. I've never seen anything like
what's going on with the iPhone right now and the
addiction, including my beloved young children
who are not here right now-- unbelievable addiction to
needing to look at that email, hit that latest headline,
what happened on Facebook? What is going on? And then I can go on and on. I was riding the train-- 90% of the people looking down. Airport-- 90% of the
people looking down. People walking into poles. Something dramatic is going
on here, and I don't know. For some reason, I'm
feeling like maybe you're throwing a little
lifeline out here. I think this is major. And I would just love
your observations on, what's going on? How is it affecting? And how the hell are
we going to deplug? And that's the short. Believe me, that's the short. JUDSON BREWER: Help me, mindful,
when you're my only hope. Yes. So I could-- I'll say something
completely self-referential, which is I wrote
a whole chapter-- several chapters
about that in my book, because I agree with you. It's crazy. And there's this great Ira Glass
episode of This American Life where he's talking about these-- he's talking to
these pre-teens who are talking about how they have
to market themselves and check their Instagram pictures
before they post them. And their job every morning is
to wake up and just go, like, like, like, like,
like, like, like, like, like for all their friends,
like that's their job, and then they wake
up the next morning. Because they're all expecting
everybody to do that. So you don't actually have to
read the chapters in my book. I'll just tell you
what they're about. And the idea here is there's
this perfect storm set up with these. Somebody described it
once as the weapons of mass distraction. I love that-- these weapons
of mass distraction. So not only do we have constant
availability of distraction, we've never ever
had that before. So there's one piece. Another piece is self-referent. So we get to talk
about ourselves in all different ways-- Twitter, Facebook. It's called YouTube. It should be called
me tube, right? So we get to post videos
and get all this stuff, talk about ourselves. That's sticky. That's rewarding. Gossip is sticky. And then you bring all these
together with the formula that the casinos
use to get people addicted to gambling,
which is called intermittent reinforcement. So we never know. So if you have your
alerts on your phones, you never know when you're
going to get that next alert. And that is the most rewarding
type of learning known. That's what we use to
get addicted to anything. So you don't know when that
next beep, or bing, or text, or tweet is going to be there. So you bring all this together--
the constant availability, all the self-related stuff, the
intermittent reinforcement, and you've got these
weapons of mass distraction. It's really interesting. I'll stop on the
social commentary, but I'll say one thing. People are asking me,
well, what about kids? What's going to happen to kids? Kids learn from their parents. And parents are learning
to regulate their emotions this way as compared to
dealing with their emotions. And so when they
don't have this thing, or this thing doesn't
work, their head explodes, because they
don't know how to handle-- and then they throw tantrums. And then, what are
their kids learning? So I think there is hope
in a sense of understanding how our minds work. If we can really
understand how they work, then we can actually fight
back against the guys who are spending sleepless
nights at Facebook to try to get us more addicted, right? AUDIENCE: That was 60 Minutes. JUDSON BREWER: Yeah,
so Anderson Cooper is very interested in this. He just did that thing. AUDIENCE: There's a link
to that site at Anderson, and he did the other story. It was amazing. JUDSON BREWER: Yeah, that
just came out recently where he's talking about-- where the industry
won't talk about it. So they're doing everything
they can to engineer. And so all you need is
a large sample size, so you need a large population. It's because-- how many billion? There are more users than there
are Catholics on the earth. That was one statistic
that I saw a while ago. They would actually-- there
are probably no other marks to surpass, because there are a
lot of Catholics on the earth. So you can take this huge
sample and in a half a day, you can do A/B testing
where you can say, is this more, or
this more addictive? And then you can turn
around, and do another one, and do another. And they're constantly
experimenting. Most major websites that we go
to were doing an experiment, so that's the way it works. So this isn't to
scare us and say we shouldn't use the internet,
because we're not going to stop using the internet. But if we can understand
and see how this works, then we can start
to regain control. Instead of being the
slaves to these things, we can ask ourselves,
what do I get from this? And start to step back and
then bring choice back in as compared to habitual reaction. So thanks for
asking the question. And hopefully-- I agree. I don't know what else
we're going to do, because they're tapping into the
strongest learning processes. If we don't tap into those,
it's not even a contest. So we have to tap into
those, and understanding them, and awareness. Our brains know how to learn. And they're going to
see really clearly, oh, this isn't
actually that great. And then we're back in
control, and then we apply it to everything. Yeah. So sorry to go on
and on about that. But I think it's
really incredible. AUDIENCE: I think you're
really on to a major frontier. I think this is much
bigger than people are even aware of 20
years down the road. We have no idea
the ramifications of where this could lead. Seriously, it's a big deal. JUDSON BREWER: It is a bit. Yeah. Yes, thank you. Thank you. Yes, please. AUDIENCE: Hi, you said that
the neurofeedback technology is obviously not a substitute
for having a meditation teacher. That doesn't seem
so obvious to me. And I'm wondering if you
can say a little bit more about why it seems
obvious to you. JUDSON BREWER: Oh, sure. Well, I've been
thinking about it a lot. So it probably-- so thank
you for asking the question. The analogy here is you all
probably heard the idea of-- if you give an infinite
number of monkeys an infinite number
of typewriters, we get Shakespeare
by sheer chance. So feedback-- I think we could
hook our heads up to these feedback machines
and eventually, we're going to trip into states
that we're like, wow, that's-- oh, let me reproduce that. But I think the
critical piece here is we don't need to wait for that. Because these practices
have been honed and actually individualized across a bunch
of different traditions, even within traditions to even
personalize these practices for people over
thousands of years. So we can actually bring
those two things together and optimize
delivery of training. So we can bring a good teacher
together with neurofeedback in a way that the teacher
can give some instruction, and the student could even
be doing a practice with neurofeedback to
see if they're-- I hate to say this, but
practice incorrectly. But here we don't know what
correct practice is, just by random chance. So here, if we
bring in something, we can actually drive the
process much more quickly and efficiently. And I haven't seen anything
better than teachers be able to do that. So that's why I'm-- so I
didn't mean to say, oh, it should be intuitively obvious
to the casual observer here. So I'm glad you
asked the question. Here I think we can bring the
best of both worlds together, which is really skilled teachers
who come up through traditions who have learned these
practices and developed their own wisdom
around that, and then bring that even together
with technology, because technology is only
hopefully getting better. Yeah. So thank you for the question. Please. AUDIENCE: Hi. I know almost nothing
about your field. I'm here because my
girlfriend wanted me to listen to you speak,
and I'm glad that I did. JUDSON BREWER: I
honor your honestly. AUDIENCE: I know. I say it because it's actually--
it's important to the question. I don't say it to
flaunt my ignorance. When you were talking
about things getting into neural
mechanisms on purpose, I had actually
heard that before. I have a friend who-- he makes video games. And a lot of what you said at
the beginning of this speech-- I had literally heard him
say it, and he hates it. He absolutely hates
that about his industry. He loves video games, and he
would prefer that they be more, I guess, pure, which is
perhaps a little childish. But I was wondering, given that
the field is very much aware of the mechanisms that it's
tapping into-- and it is doing it on purpose-- have you ever thought about
having a meeting of minds, a crossing of
disciplines, perhaps, with people like my
friend who understand how to use those things,
but would prefer that games bring people joy
and do distract them from things in a good way-- it's essentially to make it so
that they're healthier and not quite as destructive to the
minds of the young people that play them? JUDSON BREWER: I think that's a
great question, because there's no question that people are
going to play video games, whether they're 10 years
old, or 30, or 50 years old. We see this. It doesn't stop when
you're suddenly an adult. So I haven't had
those conversations. I certainly welcome them. I know that we've been
contacted a fair amount by-- in particular, people are really
jazzed about virtual reality right now. I've tended to decline those
types of relationships. We're trying to just get people
to pay attention to reality. So we're going to start
there, and we'll see. I mean, VR is not going
away, so eventually we will. We can't ignore that. We can't be Luddites
in that sense, but there I don't see
a clear connection with virtual reality. But I think with
the gaming industry, it would be very interesting to
see, what intrinsically joyful things can we tap
into with video games? So on a very simple level. And it's not a really
simplistic-- but simple. If we can train people to
notice this versus this, we've now calibrated this
machine as a feedback machine, and we don't need
any fancy technology. So if a video game
could help people-- because video games
are doing this. They're trying to get people
dopamine, dopamine, dopamine. So if somebody could tap
into the intrinsic rewards-- you would think if they could
do that well, they'd actually make a video game that
would outsell the others, because this ultimately is
more intrinsically rewarding. That being said, when
people tap into this, they realize they don't
need a video game to do it. So it might be-- so I think there are
pieces there that are going to be interesting. Because ultimately, it's
about ending all addiction in that sense. And when we end addiction,
we're not dependent on something that we're going to pay for. And that makes it
challenging for people that are looking to
make a lot of money from this to want
to do those things. So there's an inherent
tension there, but I would welcome those
types of conversations to see. At least it's a start. How can we train
this in a video game? That would be awesome. So thank you for the question. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] JUDSON BREWER: Yeah. Great, thank you. AUDIENCE: Thank
you for your talk. I was pretty excited by a few
of the therapeutic avenues that you're working on. JUDSON BREWER: Step up a little. There. AUDIENCE: I just
wanted to ask, are you worried about off-target
effects as far as this goes? I mean, all of the focus
seems to be on the posterior cingulate cortex. And that same region is also
involved in activation when people think about loved ones. You only focused on negative
things it gets activated in. Oddly enough, the example
you used-- the art. I quickly looked. [INAUDIBLE] visual art also
activates people's posterior cingulate cortex. JUDSON BREWER: Visual art? AUDIENCE: Yeah. So is there a worry that
if we're decreasing that? That loop is going to be
involved in good things, too, right? Feedback loop lets me know. When I make fun of
my girlfriend's shirt and she gets mad, don't
make fun of it again, right? So are we targeting
specifically through mindfulness the part of the
posterior cingulate cortex that is somehow
activated in my consuming food? Or am I going to be decreasing
seemingly on the diagram-- we're looking to decrease the
entire activation of that loop, which is an important loop? JUDSON BREWER: Yeah,
it's a good question. And part of this is-- so the default mode network
gets activated quite a bit with a lot of things. And I've seen more than
a couple of studies where they hit this fallacy of
correlation equals causation, and that's problematic. So we've seen, for example,
studies where people-- they correlate default
mode network activation with creativity. And so on average,
we're getting lost-- 50% of the wake-life-- 47%. So 50% of waking-life, we could
either be aware or not aware. And if we happen to have
some creative moment-- OK, creative moment pops up. By random chance, we could
associate that with something being self-referential or not. And then we associate
correlation with causality. So I think we have to be
really careful about that. And that's where the
neurophenomologic studies are really important. So for example,
during art, somebody could be looking
at a picture of art and then thinking about
themselves, right? So it doesn't mean
that the default mode network is associated with
aesthetics or something like that. That's where we have to do
really, really careful science. And in fact, with the
default mode network, it's been shown that increased
PCC activity decreases performance, et cetera,
et cetera, et cetera. So I have not seen studies
showing that the default mode network in the PCC in
particular is particularly beneficial in one sense. And somebody actually
said, can you just zap that part of my brain? Not sure I would do that. But we can start to see this
caught-upness versus this. And this may actually be
a timestamp that links us, so that the conceptual
sense of self with events. And so that's probably helpful. And it's like, I almost
get hit by a bus. Bam! Contraction. And then I remember,
OK, look both ways before I cross the street. So I think-- I'm not saying
that the default mode network is all good or all bad. I'm just saying if we understand
what it's actually doing, then we're going to have much
more power to work with it. So for example, that
timestamp of kind of having a sense of self-- probably helpful. When we get caught up in
that and we get caught up in rumination, for example-- less helpful. So that same mechanism
kind of spins on itself. Does that kind of
get at your question? AUDIENCE: I suppose. Is there any fear
specifically, sort of off-target affects something? Maybe a more similar
example being there are moments where you're
excited about being distracted, like your child surprises you at
your house-- you didn't expect, and you get all excited. Are you worried
about the reverse happening there
that you're mindful, and you think that
this isn't something I need to be associated with-- you're separated
from that moment? Or do you feel like the
therapy is separated? And then, how do you get
that to actually show up in your mechanism? Because currently
that isn't showing in the mechanistic explanation. JUDSON BREWER: You're
talking about separated? Describe that a little bit more. AUDIENCE: Just separated out. What would be positive
examples of the activation of these regions versus
negative examples? So either the
therapy has to lose in these positive
examples, or we're missing a mechanistic step
where mindfulness allows us to choose when to be mindful. JUDSON BREWER: So let me
make sure I understand. So for example, there's
a level of connectedness that arises when we're not
thinking about ourselves all the time. Generosity is a simple example. When we're truly acting
in a selfless manner, there's a feeling of expansion. So we can all think of times
when we've truly just done something generously
and not looking for something in return. That type of thing is correlated
with decreased default mode network activity. So there's a positive--
so I think of connection as a positive piece of this. And you brought up love. There's some really
nice studies. Actually-- mentioned
some of them in my book around
differentiating out maternal and even long-term,
stable, romantic love. So the reward-based
learning pieces with the ventral tegmental
area and the nucleus accumbens-- those
dopamine-- they're probably more opioid in that sense. But there's a rewarding
quality with romantic love that can actually be
separated out by the level that somebody is obsessed
with their romantic partner. So the level of obsession
correlates with increased PCC activity, whereas folks that
are not kind of caught up in that stable
relationship-- they still have the joy that comes
with being with their partner. And they're still activating
the nucleus accumbens types of reward systems, but
that's separated from the PCC. So I think these
self-referential networks-- I might have been
confusing folks in terms of saying
self-referential equals reward. What we're seeing is
the reward pathways. And those can link up
with self-reference, but they don't
necessarily have to. So we can have the
reward of love. And there are these
types of love, so just agape is
different than eros. And they actually
can differentiate these with their own imaging
now, where the eros-- the contracted
quality of, I want. The beginning of a
romantic relationship looks a lot like addiction. So we can actually start to
separate out that contracted quality of the
experience of love from the joyful, flowing,
connected quality as well. So maybe those are some more
examples where we can actually separate out the
positive qualities that come from the connectedness. That would decrease default
mode network activity. But I really love your question. I'll have to think more
about that as well. AUDIENCE: Thank you. JUDSON BREWER: Yeah, thank you. SPEAKER 5: If there
are no other questions, please join me in
thanking Dr. Brewer.