7. Layered Knowledge Representations

Video Statistics and Information

Video
Captions Word Cloud
Reddit Comments
Captions
The following content is provided under a Creative Commons license. Your support will help MIT OpenCourseWare continue to offer high quality educational resources for free. To make a donation or to view additional materials from hundreds of MIT courses, visit MIT OpenCourseWare at ocw.mit.edu. MARVIN MINSKY: What do you think AI people should work on next? AUDIENCE: There were lots of questions I was going to ask you before you said your question. I was going to ask you, what kind of questions do you think that AI people should be asking right now? MARVIN MINSKY: That's right. Anybody have a meta question? One good question is-- oops. I could focus it better or make it bigger. Is that enough layers? It's possible that I got the idea from Sigmund Freud. Who knows what Sigmund Freud's three layers were? Sure. AUDIENCE: Id, ego, super-ego. MARVIN MINSKY: I can't here. AUDIENCE: Id, ego, super-ego. MARVIN MINSKY: Freud wrote about 30 books. I know because I had a graduate student once who decided to quit computer science and go into emergency medicine. He had an MD, which he wasn't using, and he suddenly got fed up with computer science. So he sold me his set of Freud books, which is-- But in Freud's vision, these don't seem to be a stack. But there is this thing, which is basic instincts. I shouldn't say basic-- maybe, to a large extent, built in. And this is learned socially. And I think the nice thing about Freud's concept, which as far as I know, doesn't appear much in earlier psychology, is that these conflict. And when a child grows up, a lot of what we call civilization or socialization or whatever comes from taking the built-in instincts-- which is, if you see something you like, take it-- and the social constraints that say, you should negotiate. And if you want something someone else has, you should fool them into wanting to give it to you, or whatever. So in fact, when I make this big stack of mechanisms, that really-- well, that's actually not the organization that the book starts to develop chapter by chapter, which was, instincts, learned reactions, and so forth, up to self-conscious. What's the next to top layer? I've forgotten. AUDIENCE: Ideals? MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah, I guess so. It's hard to tell. The reason I had six layers is that, unlike what people do when making theories in science, is, I assume that whatever ideas I have, they're not enough. That is, instead of trying to reduce the mind to as few mechanisms as possible, I think you want to leave room. If your theory is to live for a few years, you want to leave room for new ideas. So the last three layers, beginning with reflective thought and self-reflective and the ideals, it's hard to imagine any clear boundaries. And at some point, when you make your own theory, maybe you can squeeze it into these extra boxes that I have. A little later in the book, another hierarchy appears. This is imagining, as a functional diagram, to show how knowledge or skills or abilities or whatever you want to call them, the things that people do, might be arranged. And as far as I know, because virtually every psychologist in the last 100 years has suffered from physics envy-- they want to make something like Newton's laws-- the result has been that-- except for the work of people like Newell and Simon and other pioneers who started research in artificial intelligence, you've heard me complain about neuroscientists, but there's pretty much the same complaint to be leveled against most cognitive scientists, who, for example, try to say, maybe the entire human mind is a collection of rules, if-then rules. Well, in some sense, you could make anything out of if-then rules. But the chances are, if you tried to make a learning machine that just tried to add rules in some beautifully general fashion, I suspect the chances are it would learn for a while and then get stuck. And indeed, that's what happened maybe five or six times in the history of artificial intelligence. Doug Lenat's system, called AM, Automated Mathematician, was a wonderful PhD thesis. And it learned arithmetic completely by itself. He just set up the thing biased so that it would have some concept of numbers, like 6 is the successor of 5 and stuff like that. And it fooled around and discovered various regularities. I think I mentioned it the other day. It first developed the concept of number, which wasn't very hard, because he wrote the whole thing in the AI language called Lisp. How many of you know Lisp? That's a lot. In looking at the biographies of the late John McCarthy all week, there were lots of attempts by the writers to say what Lisp was. [LAUGHTER] The tragedy is, you probably could have described it exactly in a paragraph. Because it's saying that each structure has two parts, and each of those has two parts. I don't remember seeing any attempt to say something about this programming language in-- yes. AUDIENCE: How do you imagine the layers interact with each other? MARVIN MINSKY: Those? AUDIENCE: Yeah, those and the six layers. MARVIN MINSKY: I think they're layers of organization. Yes, because a trance frame is made of two frames. But then, if there were a neuroscientist who said, oh, maybe when you see an apple and you're hungry, you reach out and eat it, so you could think of that as a simple reflex-- if this, then that. Or you could say, if I'm hungry now and I want to be not hungry, what's a possible action-- could I do? And that action might be, look for an apple. Yes. AUDIENCE: So I was teaching my class on the day that McCarthy passed away, and then I was explaining. And then I had some students in the class who weren't even computer scientists, so I was thinking about the same problem, which is, how to explain what Lisp was? And so I said, well, before that, there were languages like Fortran that manipulated numbers. Lisp was the first language to manipulate ideas. MARVIN MINSKY: Mm-hmm. Yes, you can manipulate representations of-- yes, it's manipulating the ideas that are represented by these expressions. And one thing that interests me is, another analog between psychology and modern cognitive psychology and computer science or artificial intelligence, is the idea of a goal. What does it mean to have a goal ? And you could say, it's a piece of machinery which says that, if there's a situation-- what you have now-- and if you have a representation of some future thing-- what you want. So of course, at anytime, you're in a situation that your brain is somehow representing maybe five or ten ways, not just one. But what does it mean to have a goal? And what it means is to have two representations. One is a representation you've made of some structure which says what things are like now. And the other is some representation of what you want. I don't think I need that. And the important thing is, what are the differences? And instead of saying, what's the difference, maybe it's good to say, what are the differences? So what does it mean to have a goal. It means to have some piece of machinery turned on. You can imagine a goal that you don't have, right? Like I can say, what's David's goal? It's to get people to go to that meeting. So what you want is to minimize the differences between what you have and what you want. And so there has to be a machinery which does what? It picks out one of these differences and tries to get rid of it. And how can you get rid of it? There's two ways. The good way is to change the situation so that difference disappears. The other way is to say, oh, that would take a year. I should give up that goal. I'm digressing. So you want something that removes-- the feedback has to go this way. Let's change the world. And get rid of that difference. Well, how do you get rid of a difference? That depends on-- maybe you have a built-in reflex. Like if you have too much CO2 in your blood, something senses it and tells you to breathe. So you've got built-in things. And the question is, how do you represent these sorts of things? And in fact, I think I got a lot of this idea from the PhD thesis of Patrick Winston, who was here a minute ago. [LAUGHTER] But the question is, how do you represent what you want and how do you represent what you have? And I think the big difference between people and other primates and reptiles and amphibians-- reptiles, fish, and going back to plants and so forth-- is that we have these very high-level powerful ways of representing differences between things. And this enables us to develop reflexes for getting rid of the differences. So this is what I think might be a picture of how the brain is organized. And at every level, these things are made of neurons. But you shouldn't be looking at the neurons individually to see how the brain works. It's like looking at a computer and saying, oh, I understand that. All I have to do is know a great deal about how each transistor works. The great thing about a computer is that it doesn't matter how the transistor works. The important thing is to recognize, oh, look, they usually come in pairs-- or really four or 10 or whatever-- called a flip-flop. Do you ever see a neuroscientist saying, where are the flip-flops in this or that? It's very strange. It's as though they're trying to develop a very powerful computer without using any concepts from computer science. It's a marvelous phenomenon. And you have to wonder, where did they grow up and how did they stay isolated? AUDIENCE: In the biology department, I think. MARVIN MINSKY: In biology departments. AUDIENCE: Or psychology departments. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, I started out in biology, pretty much. And then I ran across these early papers, one of which was by Lettvin and McCulloch and Pitts and people like that. In the early 1940s, the idea of symbolic neurons appeared. It had first appeared in 1895 or so with the paper that Sigmund Freud wrote but couldn't get published. I think I mentioned that, called Project for a Scientific Psychology. And it had the idea of neurons with various levels of activation. And sometimes you would have a pair of them. And one would be inhibiting the other, and so that could store some information. And he's not very explicit about how these things might work. But as far as I know, it's about the first attempt to have a biological theory of information processing at all. And he was unable to get it published. AUDIENCE: Marvin? MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah. AUDIENCE: Since McCarthy did this, just, do you some reflections on the stuff that he did or his contributions? MARVIN MINSKY: There are a lot of things that he did. I noticed that none of the obituaries actually had any background. What had happened is that what Newell and Simon, they had struggled to make programs that could do symbolic logic. And they made a language called IPL. And IPL was a language of very microscopic operations. Like you have several registers, put a symbol in a register, perhaps do a piece of arithmetic on two symbols if they're numbers. If not, link up-- you can sort of make registers artificially in the memory of the computer. And you could take two or three registers. It had instructions for making lists or trees. So you could arrange these in a way that-- so here are the three things. And this is just a simple list, but I've drawn it as though these two were subsidiary to that. Of course, that depends on what program is going to look at this. And you could have a program which can say, here are two arguments for a function. And it doesn't matter what order they're in. It just depends how you wrote the function. So Newell and Simon had written a programming language which said, put something in a register, link it to another register, and then perform the usual arithmetic operations. In fact, what they were doing is mostly performing logical operations, because even the early computers had ANDs and ORs and XORs, and things like that. So Newell and Simon had written a program that could deal with Boolean functions and prove little theorems about, not A or B. Is this not A or not B? Is that wrong? I forget. It should have more nots. Anyway, they wrote this beautiful but clumsy thing made of very simple logical primitives. McCarthy, at the same time, IBM had spent 400 man years writing a program called Fortran 1, or Fortran. I'm not sure that people had serial numbers on programming languages yet, because we're talking about the middle 1950s. And McCarthy had been thinking about, how would you make AI programs that could do symbolic reasoning? And he was indeed particularly interested in logic. Backtrack-- in fact, Newell and Simon had got their program to find proofs of most of the theorems in the first volume of Russell and Whitehead's Principia Mathematica, which is a huge two-volume book from about 1905, I think, which was the first successful attempt to reduce mathematics to logic. And they managed to get up to calculus and show that differentiation and integration can be expressed as logical functions and variables. It's a great tour de force, because logic itself barely existed. Bool and a few others, including Leibniz, had invented Boolean algebra-like things and around-- Frege and others had got predicate calculus with their [INAUDIBLE]. And that stuff was just appearing. And Russell and Whitehead wrote this huge book, which got all the way up to describing continuous and differentiable functions and so forth. The first volume was huge and just did propositional calculus, which Aristotle had done some of, also. And anyway, McCarthy looked at that and said, why can't there be a functional language like Fortran that can do symbolic reasoning. And pretty much all by himself, he got the basic ideas from Lisp. They're built on the Newell and Simon experiment. But he basically converted the symbolic system into something like Fortran, which was only able to manipulate numbers into Lisp, which was able to manipulate arbitrary symbols in various ways. And if you want to know more about that, you can find McCarthy's home page at Stanford. I think if you Google search for McCarthy and SAIL-- SAIL is Stanford Artificial Intelligence Laboratory-- and you'll get his home page. And there's a 1994 article about how he invented Lisp. Yes. AUDIENCE: Right after you started talking about Lisp, you jumped over and you said, here's the now and the want, which is the current state of desire. And I think you were going for some kind of analogy between the symbolic evaluation [INAUDIBLE] and what conscious entities do. However, one of the most beautiful things about Lisp is the homo [INAUDIBLE],, the fact that you have things such as macros. What is there in the conscious entity that is equivalent to the macro? MARVIN MINSKY: Macros? AUDIENCE: Yeah. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, each of these levels are ways of combining things at the previous level. The way I drew it, at the top, there are stories. What's a story? You mention some characters in a typical story. Then you say, and here's a problem these characters encountered. And here's what Joe did to solve the problem, but it didn't work and here are the bugs. The reason that didn't work is that Mary was in the way, so he killed her. And then blah, blah, blah. So that's what stories are. If you just write a series of sentences, it's not a story, even though The New Yorker managed to make those into stories for about a period of 20 years. But a typical story introduces a scene and it introduces a problem, and then it produces a solution. But the solution has a bug. And then the rest of it is how you get around that bug and how you maybe change the goals in the worst case. So a story is a series of incidents. I wonder if I brought my death ray. It looks like I didn't. Oh, but you don't need it with this. You have to be here. Anyway, but what's a story made of? Well, there's situations. And you do something and you got a new situation. So what's that? That's what this thing I called a trance frame, which is a pair of situations. OK, so what's an individual situation? Well, it's a big network of nodes and relationships. And I'm not sure why I have semantic networks down here rather than here. Oh, well, a frame is a collection of representations that's sort of stereotyped or canned, that might have a single word like-- oh, just about any word, breaking something. If I say, John broke the X, you immediately say, oh, that's a trance frame. Here is an object that had certain properties, parts, and relationships. And it's been replaced by this thing which has most of the same objects, but different relationships, or one of the objects is missing and it's been replaced by another frame, and so forth. So one question is, what's the relation? So this is a picture of cognitive representations. Everything is made of little parts. And in the society of mind, I described the idea of K-line lines. What's a K-line line? It's an imaginary structure in the nervous system. There's really two kinds. There's a sort of perceptual K-line line, which is something that recognizes that, say, 10 features of a situation are present. And if these are all present, then a certain bunch of neurons or neural activity goes on. And on the other hand, when you think of something, like a word, suppose I say microphone, then you're likely to think of something that has a business end which collects sounds. And it has a stand or maybe it has a handle. And if you're an engineer, you know that probably it has a battery or a transmitter or a wire. So those are the things that I call frames or K-line lines, really. And anyway, chapter 8 of The Emotion Machine talks about that. And there's a lot of detail in the old book, The Society of Mind, about what K-line lines and things like that could be made of. Now, whatever those are, and as far as I know, no matter how hard you look, you won't find any published theories of, what in the nervous system is used to represent the things above that sort of midline there of cortical columns. I suspect that almost everything that the human brain does, that fish and those lower animals or earlier animals, I should say-- I'm not sure that higher and lower makes any sense-- are probably symbolic processes, where it probably does more harm than good to have elaborate theories of, what's the physiological chemistry of neurons. But at some point, we want to know what, in fact, the brain is made of. The cortical column has a few hundred brain cells arranged. And there are several projects around the world trying to take electron microscopes and pieces of mouse brain or cat brain and make a huge connection matrix. The problem is that the electron microscope, even the electron microscope pictures still aren't good enough to show you at each synapse what is probably going on. Eventually, people will get theories of that and get slightly better instruments. And I'm not sure that the present diagrams that they're producing are going to be much use to anyone. Yes-- some nice questions. AUDIENCE: This question might be a little bit difficult. So I'm going to start from the goal being a difference between the now state and the desired state. MARVIN MINSKY: How do we reduce the difference? Yeah. AUDIENCE: Yeah, how do we get the difference? And now, I'm going mean also take the fact of this, that the animals, they're different. And I'd say that one of the biggest big differences is that we, as people, we can describe these differences. MARVIN MINSKY: It actually is only good for the recording. AUDIENCE: OK, so we can describe these differences and talk about them with other people without having to act on those goals or hypothetical goals. So if everything that has to do with the goals is represented with a complex structure like this one, and I want to implement it in a computer, well, for every hypothetical case that has to do with the solving of a certain problem, I can just make more copies in the memory. But if I have a human brain, then I don't know how convenient it is to postulate that there is like a huge memory databank where you can make copies of everything that's going on, or if you have to assume some kind of a huge collection of pointers and acting on those pointers. So this is just a random point, and I'd like to hear if you have any ideas on this. MARVIN MINSKY: That's an important question. We know quite a bit about the functions of some parts of the brain. I don't think I've ever tried to draw a brain. But there's a structure called the amygdala. How do you spell it? I believe that means almond-shaped. Is that right? Anybody have a google handy? And that's down here somewhere. And it has the property that it contains the short-term memories. So anything that's happened in the last couple of seconds is somehow represented here in a trenchant way. And everything that's happened in the last 20 minutes or so leaves traces in the amygdala, so that if somebody is in an automobile accident or is knocked out by a real powerful boxing punch, then when you wake up later, you can't remember anything that happened in about 20 minutes or a half hour before the trauma. So that's a very good experiment to try. And there's a lot of evidence that this happens because the memories of the last half hour or so are stored in-- the conjecture is in a dynamic form. Maybe there are huge numbers of loops of neurons connected in circles, or circle. Anyway, nobody really knows. But if you have a bunch of neurons connected in a big circle, maybe 20 or 30 of them, then you can probably put 10 or 20 bits of information in form of different spaced pulses. And a great mystery would be, how does the brain manage to maintain that particular pattern of pulses for 10 or 20 minutes? Then no one knows where it goes after the 20 minutes. But if a person gets enough sleep that night, then it turns out that it's no longer in the amygdala and it's somewhere else in the brain. And so one question is, if there's all this stuff stored here-- and you might think of it as what's happened in the primary memory. Every computer starts out with-- the first computers had just two or three registers. Then they got 16 and 32. And I imagine-- how many fast registers are there in a modern computer? I haven't been paying attention, maybe 64, whatever. Anyway, but the next day, if you've gotten some sleep, you can retrieve them. Somehow they're copied somewhere else. As far as I know, there is no theory of how the brain decides where to put them and how it records it and so forth. There's something very strange about this big science lacking theories, isn't there? What would you do if you were in a profession where they're talking about dozens and dozens of mechanisms which clearly exist, and you say, how do you think that works, and they say, blah, I don't know. None of my friends know either, so I guess it's OK. [LAUGHTER] So we don't know how it picks a place to put them. And we don't know how, when you ask a question, it gets back there so that you can reprocess it and talk about it. But anyway, so I made up these theories that things are stored in the form of K-line lines. And there's a lot of discussion in The Society of Mind about how K-line lines probably have to work and so forth. And if you look at The Society of Mind in Amazon, you'll find this enraging review by a neurologist named Robert [INAUDIBLE],, who says he introduces these undefined terms called K-line and paranomes and things like that, of which there's whole chapters in the book. AUDIENCE: I've got a question before we go on. MARVIN MINSKY: That's my favorite review of explaining the problem of getting neuroscience to grow up. Yes, who had a question? Yes. AUDIENCE: One thing that puzzles me is, how does the the brain decide to do things? MARVIN MINSKY: How do they decide what to do, did you say? AUDIENCE: Well, say you want to relay a story, but like hundreds of things happened. How do you select what to tell and what not to? It seems like you need some sort of intentionality behind it, but how do we learn to do that in the first place, almost? Like when you describe a goal, you describe what's going on now and what is the thing you want. But then how do you decide which few things to be considered? MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, you're asking a wonderful question. Which is, after all, if I were to talk to you for an hour about your goals, you could tell me hundreds of goals that you have. So the question is, how do you pick the one that you're thinking about now. AUDIENCE: Yeah. Also, how do you represent the priority of goals? They're almost described as like [INAUDIBLE] machines, as turned on all the time. But how do you resolve conflicts? How do you decide which one to go first and which ones [INAUDIBLE]? MARVIN MINSKY: OK, how do you represent the relations of your goals? The standard theory, for which there's no evidence, is that-- if I could use the word hierarchy. So if you ask a naive person, they'll give you a pretty good theory-- completely wrong, but good. And the standard theory says, well, there's one big goal at the top. And you say, what is it? And some people would say, well, maybe it's to reproduce. Darwin could, but didn't argue that. Or to survive or something like that. OK, so if you take that one, then it's obvious what the next goals in the hierarchy would be. I'm not saying this is how things work. I don't think it does. So there's food. And there's air. Air gets very high priority. If you put a pillow over somebody, their first priority is to breathe. And they don't even think about eating for a long time. [LAUGHTER] So that's very nice. I don't know what comes after that. If you're out in the cold freezing, then there's temp. The nice thing about air and temp is that, if you have those goals, you can satisfy them in parallel. Because in fact, you don't need much of a hierarchy. The breathing thing has this servo mechanism, where if there's a higher level of CO2 than normal in your blood, then, what is it, the vagus nerve? I don't know. They know a lot about which part of the brain gets excited and raises your breathing rate or your heartbeat rate. My favorite animal is the swordfish. How many of you know about brown fat? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Brown fat is a particular thing found in invertebrates. And it's fat. It's brown, I guess. And it has the property that it can be innervated by nerve fibers. And they cause it to start burning calories. And the swordfish is normally cold-blooded. But its carotid arteries have a big organ of brown fat around them just as the blood comes into the brain. And if you turn on the brown fat, it warms the brain and the IQ of the swordfish goes way up. [LAUGHTER] And it swims faster and uses better evasive tactics. And there are a couple of other cold-blooded animals that are known to have a warm-blooded brain that they can-- isn't that a great feature? I wonder, does our brain do a little bit of that? I got lost telling funny stories. So anyway, this K-line idea is very simple. It says that, perhaps the way human memory works is that, here and there you have big collections of nerve fibers that go somewhere and go to lots of cells. Let's say thousands of these cells. And each one is connected to some particular combination. Imagine there's 100 wires here. And each of these cells is connected to, say, 10 of those wires. Then how many different cells could you have for remembering different features of what's on this big bus bar? How many ways are there picking 10 things out of 100? It's about 100 to the 10th power. So here's a simple kind of memory. Of course, it'd be useless unless these bits by themselves have some correlation with some useful concept. And then at any particular time on this particular bunch of fibers in the brain, maybe 20 of these are turned on. And if something very important has happened, you send a signal to all these cells and say, any of you cells who are seeing more than 10 or 15 of these 20 fibers at this moment should remember that and set themselves to do something next time you see that pattern. Something like that, something has to decide which of these cells is going to copy at this time and so forth. But so there is a theory of how memory, kind of symbolic memory, might work in the brain. And I got this idea from a paper-- I don't remember what their idea was-- but by David Waltz and Jordan Pollack. Pollack is a theorist at Brandeis, I guess, who in recent years has turned into some kind of artist, and makes all sorts of beautiful things and simple robots that do this and that. David Waltz, search his web page sometime, because he was here for many years as a graduate student, and then developed beautiful theories of vision. When did Dave move? Do you remember, Pat? Anyway-- AUDIENCE: '79? MARVIN MINSKY: Something like that. But as far as I know-- so I made up this theory, which is really copied from Waltz and Pollack, but simplified and neatened up. Then I went on and made other theories based on that. But without them, I would have been stuck in some conditioned reflex theory for a long time, I suspect. AUDIENCE: You mentioned the role that the amygdala plays in storing the short-term memory. And you mentioned that [INAUDIBLE] the memories that are stored in there are wiped out. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, that's a question. Presumably, as you grow up, your amygdala gets better at learning what to recognize. But I've never seen any discussion or theory of how much of your short-term memory is-- how do you learn and develop and get better at remembering things that are worth remembering? Sorry, go ahead. AUDIENCE: My understanding is that, so whatever memories are stored in the amygdala during-- whatever is stored in the amygdala is wiped out after you sleep. What sort of implications does this have about remembering dreams? Because my understanding is that, after you have a lucid dream, the memories of the dream are wiped out after a certain point later on in the day. So what sort of implications does it have on the ability of people to remember something [INAUDIBLE]?? MARVIN MINSKY: Good question. There have been some theories. But I think I mentioned Freud's theory, which is that you're not remembering anything. When you wake up, you make up the dream. And I think that's surely completely wrong. But Freud made it up because he was mad at Jung. [LAUGHTER] Jung had a theory that people have telepathic connections with other people. And so he had been Freud's student or disciple for some years. And then he went mystical, and Freud went up the wall, because Jung was obviously very smart and imaginative. I'm trying to think if I've had a very good student who turned mystical-- one or two, but not like that. Anyway, that's a great question. And when I said things hang around in the amygdala for 20 minutes, that's just some things. If it takes sleep the next night, which is eight hours or 16 hours later, to solidify it or to copy it in some way into the other parts of the brain, it must still be in the amygdala or somewhere. So in fact, maybe there's the amygdala and some other parts of the brain that haven't been identified that contain slightly different copies of the memories for a longer time and so forth. So who knows where and how they work. Maybe the language centers remember paragraphs of things that you've heard or said for some time, and so forth. I don't think anybody really knows much about-- they're very sure about the amygdala, because injuring the amygdala or injecting Novocaine into a blood vessel that goes there has such a dramatic effect on-- you just can't remember anything for that short period or half hour or so. Maybe memories are stored in 10 different ways in 10 different parts of the brain. Who knows? One problem that I think I mentioned is that, although a great deal has been learned about the brain from modern scanning techniques, almost every result that people talk about is obtained by turning up the contrast so that most of the brain is dark and nerve centers that are highly active show up in your-- you've all seen these pictures which show three or four places in the brain lighting up. Well, there's a good chance that, for any particular event, there might be 10 or 20 places that have just increased a little bit. And when they turn up the contrast, all that evidence is lost because those regions all become black or whatever. AUDIENCE: Yeah, I don't know the next part of that. But I would go as far as to say that, probably they have a finding where there are no specific areas, so you have a pretty uniform picture of the changes in metabolism, then you don't make theories or you don't publish that result, because you don't have any clear areas for [INAUDIBLE],, and that nobody knows that, OK, that particular thing was actually exciting [INAUDIBLE]. That's just a guess. MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah, it could be that some things involve very large amounts of brain. But I'm inclined to doubt it. Probably you want to turn a lot of things off most of the time so they don't fill up with random garbage. Who knows? Yes. AUDIENCE: And to follow up with that, why do you think the hierarchy of goals is naive? And what specific features of goals do you think that structure doesn't achieve? MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, I didn't finish that, did I? She's asking-- I started to say, what's the hierarchy of goals? But it looks like I got stuck on the well-defined, instinctive goals that you need to stay alive. And I guess my answer is, I don't have any good theories of how you do that. At any time, when you're talking to somebody, you usually have a couple of very clear goals, like, I want to explain this, I want this other person to understand this for this reason. I'm having trouble. Maybe I have to get his or her attention by-- and then you get a sub-goal of doing some social thing to convince them to listen to you and all sorts of things. But I just don't have a nice picture. When you're writing an AI program, you usually have goals and sub-goals in a very clear arrangement. Like the theorem-proving programs are wonderful, because you've proved some kind of expression, but the particular theorem you are trying to prove has another condition which is different from this condition. And people have gone quite far in making models of something like theorem-proving, where the world is very simple. If you're proving something in geometry or group theory or a little fragment of mathematics, then there are only 5 or 10 assumptions hanging around. And so you could actually plan a little bit of exhaustive search to go through your four levels. And then you would do something like in a chess program of, over time, discovering it never pays to explore a tree that has this feature because of whatever. Yes. AUDIENCE: Are goals relevant? Like, we always have goals where it's just like something where it's like, when I play chess, maybe I have a goal, but why should I have a goal? Why isn't that like, maybe, I don't know, that goal [INAUDIBLE] at some points of my life. Why are goals important? MARVIN MINSKY: Well, the survival goals are important because if you cross the street without looking, you could do that about 20 times before you're dead. AUDIENCE: So just really the survival goals are important? MARVIN MINSKY: Well, if you don't make a living, you'll starve. So now, if you've committed yourself to being a mathematician, now you have to be a good mathematician or else you'll starve, and so forth. I was a pretty good mathematician. Only my goal was, I had to be the best mathematician, so I quit. You don't want to have a goal you can't achieve. AUDIENCE: Yeah, but is that part of [INAUDIBLE]?? MARVIN MINSKY: Well, a lot of people do have one. So it eats up a lot of their time and they're wasting it. I'm not sure what the question is. I think the feature of humans is that they're sort of general purpose. So there are a lot of things people do, which are bad things to do. You can't justify them. You can think of people as part of a huge search process. And as a species or a genetic system, it pays to have a few crazy ones every now and then. Because if the environment suddenly changes, maybe they'll be the only ones who survive. But William Calvin's question, how come people evolved intelligence so rapidly in five million years? And he attributes it partly to five-- how many periods of global cooling were there? It's about six or seven ice ages in the last-- anybody know the history of the Earth? Anyway, some evidence-- at least used to be, I haven't paid any attention-- is that the human population's got down to maybe just tens of thousands several times in the last million years. And so only the really different ones managed to pull through. It might be the one who had all sorts of useless abilities. Yes. It was the ones who ate the others, which would have been punished before that. Go ahead. AUDIENCE: You're talking about representing the K-lines and everything like [INAUDIBLE],, some lines and then activating some of these features. So in the case of learning, which have changed some of these connections? And if this is the case, how would this effect the higher order, like higher level, just like frames and [INAUDIBLE]?? Like if you change something that's really for low-level representation, will this effect a lot-- like the whole system will break because some stop procedure wouldn't be able to return properly? MARVIN MINSKY: That's great. That's another question I can't begin to answer. Namely, when you learn something-- let's take the extreme form-- do you start a new representation or you modify an old one? OK, that's a choice. If you modify an old one, can you do that without losing the previous version? So for example, if I grow up monolingually, which I did-- so I learned English. And then I can't remember why, but for some reason, around 4th grade, some teacher tried to teach us German. And so for each German word, I try very hard to find the equivalent English word and figure out how to move your mouth so that it comes out German, which actually, for many words, works fine, because English is a mixture of German and other things. And for many words, it doesn't. So that was a bad idea. [LAUGHTER] And if I had gotten very good at it, I could have lost some English. Anyway, that's a great set of questions. When do you make new memories? When do you modify old ones? And the hard one is, if you can't modify an old one, how do you make a copy that's different. And there's a section called, in Society of Mind, about how we do that in language by paraphrasing what someone said. Or you say something in your own head and you misunderstand it. I'm doing that all the time. I say, such and such is a this or a that. And then it's as though somebody else had said that. And I have someone going, no, that's wrong, he meant this. So when you're talking to yourself, you're actually converting some mysterious inarticulate representation to speech. And then you're running it through your brain and listening to it, and converting the speech back to a new representation. So I think the wonderful thing about language is, it's not just for expressing. How come the only animal that thinks the way we do is the only animal that talks the way we do? Who knows? Maybe whales, but nobody has decoded them do something like that. But that's a question. How do you copy a memory to make a new version? And the answer is, you can ask anyone and they'll say, I don't know. Why aren't there five theories of that? Yeah. AUDIENCE: I don't know if that's true, but I believe there is sort of an objective path between words in language-- like for example, know, no. So for example, if I have a table, I say [SPEAKING PORTUGUESE] in Portuguese. There is sort of an objective that-- even for colors, which is sort of weird, because you're kind of dividing all colors into [INAUDIBLE] of colors. And even, I believe, for words, languages that were not formed from the same ancestry. I don't know. For me, it seems that there's some sort of objective [INAUDIBLE] between words. AUDIENCE: Well, there are certain things that are just sort of naturally more useful to talk about, and so a lot of languages can have words for the same thing. Like languages will have a word that means table. But if there are some cultures that don't have tables, that they probably wouldn't have a single word for table. And regarding the colors thing, there have actually been some interesting studies done on that. Like they've done color-naming surveys with lots of languages in the world. And it turns out that different languages partition the color space differently. MARVIN MINSKY: I was just curious if anybody knows it. AUDIENCE: They tend to do it similarly. The way that people perceive colors, the way that they divide up the space based on the number of words that they have is actually like maximizing the similarity of things with the same name and the difference between [INAUDIBLE].. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] did some interesting studies on that. MARVIN MINSKY: Right. I'm trying to remember when children use colors. Let's see. I had a daughter-- who I have still, I must say-- and she suddenly started color words, and had six or seven of them in just a couple of days. And she sort of got interested in that. And so suddenly, I said, oh, my gosh, this is what Maria Montessori calls a-- I forget what she called it. What's this moment when the child is open to being taught? So I said, this is her chance to learn a lot of new color names. So I said, and what about this? I said that's aqua. And she said, that's blue-green. [LAUGHTER] And I said, no, this is aqua. And she refused. So that was that. So some months later, she learned some more color names, but it wasn't the same. AUDIENCE: When she was saying blue-green, is that because somebody had taught her blue-green, or it was because she was combining those two colors? MARVIN MINSKY: I think she was combining it. Do you remember? MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah, I I think she was combining it, because it looked a little blue and a little green. AUDIENCE: So she had a concept that there were certain building blocks of colors. MARVIN MINSKY: It looked like she wouldn't accept a new one. [LAUGHTER] And I always wondered if I was 15 minutes too late. When does the Montessori door shut? AUDIENCE: Sort of similarly, there are studies done on different languages of how well people distinguish different colors. So in Russian, there's a different word that means light blue and dark blue, so Russians are better at distinguishing different shades of blue than, say, Americans. And there's also a lot of languages that don't have two distinct words for blue and green, and native speakers of those languages have trouble distinguishing blues and greens. MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, that gives me a great idea. If I could have found some unfamiliar objects and colored them aqua, that might have fooled her. [LAUGHTER] Is there a branch of psychology that worries about-- of course, there must be names for sounds too. And certainly, a lot's known about ages at which children can't get new phonemes. Yes, Henry. AUDIENCE: I've got a story in Memory and Language. So I'm bilingual. Maybe other bilingual people in the audience can confirm this. MARVIN MINSKY: I didn't know that. What's your other language? AUDIENCE: French. MARVIN MINSKY: For heaven's sake. I've known Henry for 20-odd years. So AUDIENCE: One think that can happen is, you can be talking with another bilingual person, so I can be talking to someone who also both speaks French and English, and then like a week later, I'll remember every detail about a complicated conversation. We were working on this project and we were going to meet at this and all this stuff. I remember every detail, except which language we had the conversation. AUDIENCE: Yeah, my entire family is bilingual. We sort of generally speak a mix of Russian and English all the time, so I can never remember even what language I'm speaking at the time. [LAUGHTER] AUDIENCE: Yeah, and that mystifies me. Because if we store it in a language, how could I forget what language? AUDIENCE: Well, I think there is a very simple answer to that one. You have a language that's neither English or French. And you just have a very simple [INAUDIBLE] there. And then whenever you want to express something in English or French, you would just decode, encode, whatever is the word, [INAUDIBLE] that you were having. AUDIENCE: Well, that's the question. What is that language? Do you have any thoughts on that? MARVIN MINSKY: I don't know. You remind me-- I think I mentioned this-- but I was once in a meeting in Yerevan, Armenia. And there was a translator who was practically real-time. And Francis Crick was talking. And at some point, the translator switched and he started translating from English to English. So Crick would say something and the translator would translate it into English, very well, I thought. It wasn't quite the same words. And after a while, somebody asked him why he was doing that, and he said he didn't realize he had switched. Do you think that could happen to you? AUDIENCE: I suppose. AUDIENCE: Do you think that there are other ways of translating ideas and learning them, maybe like art or music, besides language that can be really helpful? MARVIN MINSKY: I don't think there's anything nearly like language. Art is pretty good, but it's so ambiguous. Cartoons, they're awfully good. AUDIENCE: How about Lisp? [LAUGHTER] MARVIN MINSKY: What? AUDIENCE: How about Lisp? Yeah, that was a joke. MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, right, programming languages. Yes, why is mathematics so hard? I wonder if the habit of using a single letter for every variable might make it easy and hard. Who knows. Yes. You have great questions. AUDIENCE: Can you you talk about, last year you mentioned that mathematics is hard. I thought about it. MARVIN MINSKY: Say it again. AUDIENCE: Last year, you mentioned that mathematics is hard. I thought about it, and I do feel like there's an extreme lack of representations of ideas. Solving a problem, we need to identify so many things and there's so many processes that you apply to them without having a name for any of them, or like classification. Well, you have induction and deduction, that's about it-- and contradiction. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes. One feature of mathematics is completely unredundant representations. I wonder if there's some way to fix that or change it. What other activity do we have where there's absolutely no redundancy at all in the mathematical expression? So for some people it's delightful, and other people it's very hard. I mentioned Licklider, who in programming, he would have very long variable names. Sometimes they'd even be sentences, like the register in which I put the result of. And the great thing was, you could read those programs. They looked sort of stupid, but he didn't have to-- what do you call notes? He didn't have to have, exclamation point, this means that. Comments, comments. AUDIENCE: When I sit in like an [INAUDIBLE] class, I just can't make myself accept the concepts unless I can understand them algebraically and [INAUDIBLE] geometric equation. MARVIN MINSKY: What kind of math do you like? Do you do topology? AUDIENCE: I like topology. I like [INAUDIBLE]. MARVIN MINSKY: I love topology. I once was tutoring a high school student who couldn't do algebra. I don't know if I mentioned this. And it turned out he didn't know how to use parentheses. So he would have an expression, stuff like that. But if there were something in there, he didn't know what that meant. He didn't know how to match them. So I couldn't figure out why. And I ask, how come? And he said, maybe I was sick the day they explained parentheses. And so I gave him little exercises like, make them into eggs. And you see, if you make this into an egg, then this egg won't work. [LAUGHTER] And the funny thing was, he got that in five minutes, and then he didn't have any trouble with algebra the rest-- can you imagine? I've never done much tutoring but, if you can find the bug like that, it must be great fun. But I bet it doesn't happen very often. That was so funny. I couldn't imagine not-- you know? So if you don't you have language, there must be-- well, why are some people so much better at math than others? Is it just that they've not understood about five things? AUDIENCE: I feel like they have a set of things they know to go to when they face a problem. Well, that's kind of similar to your [INAUDIBLE] story. But I feel like they know exactly. They have names for concepts of like ways of solving problems. So they look like I'm trying this approach and then if it doesn't work. I know this other approach. I just try it. Instead of looking at the problem and you think, OK, so what possible thing, what possible method, can I think of using? MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, names for methods. So do you have public names or are they secret names? AUDIENCE: I feel like they are secret names. They're just like stores-- because they can't explain it to other people. They can't be like, this problem-- like very few of them-- and like this problem and that problem, they have the general same method [INAUDIBLE].. MARVIN MINSKY: I had a friend who was a composer. And she had all sorts of sounds. And they were filed away on tapes. And the tapes had little symbols on them. And if she needed a sound, she would go to the closet and pull out the right tape. And it had more symbols written on the reel. And she'd get this thing, which might be a thunderstorm or a bird or something. So I asked her what was her notation for these sounds. And she giggled and said, I can't tell you. It's too embarrassing. I never found out. [LAUGHTER] But she had developed some code for sounds. Yeah. AUDIENCE: So I would say that they have some [INAUDIBLE] representation of things that require [INAUDIBLE] symbols or patterns of solutions. And their representation is optimal. And if you're good at math, it doesn't mean that you're good at playing music. Because when you play music well, you have maybe a good representation of the sounds. And so it's just [INAUDIBLE]. And so you cannot access all-- so I have these solutions, these patterns of solutions that I need to-- I don't know. I need to solve this math problem, by deduction or by contradiction. And in his brain, or somebody that knows a lot, like the person can access very faster, their representation itself is very well-defined. So you can access [INAUDIBLE]. MARVIN MINSKY: That's an interesting whole-- let me interrupt. I was once in some class, math class. And it was about n dimensional vector spaces. And some student asked, well, how do you imagine the n dimensional vector space? Its two stories. And the instructor, who I forget who, thought for a while. Then he said, oh, it's very simple. Just pick a particular n So that was completely useless. [LAUGHTER] And I was a disciple of a mathematician at Harvard named Andrew Gleason, who was a wonderful man. Only a couple of years older than me, but he had won the Putnam three times, first prize. And I said what would you tell a student who wanted to understand an n dimensional vector space, what it means? And he said, well, you should give him five or six ways. I don't know. Like imagine a bunch of arrows. And remember that each of them is at right angles to all the others, just like that. Then he added, of if there's an infinite number, you should have the sum of the squares of their lengths converge to a finite value. And then he said, or you should think of it as a Fourier series with things of different frequencies. And then he said, or you should think of an object in a topological space, and each dimension is finding the boundary of the last one. And he went on for about six or seven, and that was a great idea. Well, have seven completely different ways. And I remember I once had the same conversation with Richard Feynman. And I said, well, how did you do that? And he said, well, when I grew up, whatever it was, I always thought of three or four representations. So if one of them didn't work, another one would. What? AUDIENCE: So my idea for somebody, if you ask them about how to understand multiple dimension space, is I'd say, read Flatland. [LAUGHTER] Because that would give you the analogy. Once you had that analogy, then it would be easy to extend it to other dimensions. MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, good. Has anybody written a 4D Flatland, where you make fun of the 3D people. They can't get out of a paper bag. [LAUGHTER] AUDIENCE: And so there will be some events that maybe will prove that. So for example, in my case, when I do a lot of math, when I try to talk to people, it's very hard. And like maybe [INAUDIBLE] my representation of solving problems in math. And people tend to get better in math if they practice it a lot, because they are optimizing their representation of math, and that would be the case. MARVIN MINSKY: I think I understand the problem, but I don't think I have any friends left who are not mathematicians. [LAUGHTER] That's what happens if you live in this place long enough. Yeah. AUDIENCE: So that's one way they're doing better. I feel like the other way they're doing better is, they objectify things that we don't objectify. It's like the learning how to learn better idea, learning how to learn better to learn better. So it's one thing to know what the right representation is for a particular problem, like the right method is. It's another thing to optimize that process. So like, what process did I use in finding that representation? And then they make that into a concept. And then they have a lot of these kinds of concepts. MARVIN MINSKY: Have you ever helped somebody to learn better. AUDIENCE: Yeah, [INAUDIBLE]. MARVIN MINSKY: What did you tell them. AUDIENCE: So she had trouble with, basically, two truth tables, something like that, like AND, OR and stuff like that. So her way of seeing a problem is to make a chart. I forget what I told her. I told her to kind of like map simple problems. MARVIN MINSKY: You know, maybe most people don't have the word representation in their language. Is there any place in grade school where you actually talk about, what's your representation of acceleration? Do we teach that word as part of any subject? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] If you're drawing some base or something like that, it'll ask you to represent it. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes. But it's hard to get out of-- yeah, OK. So they're radically different representations of-- AUDIENCE: Maybe that's [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, I don't know. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Tinker Toys. Tinker Toy. AUDIENCE: What's that? MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, to represent physical structures as Tinker Toys. Yeah, I wrote a little article complaining about the popularity of LEGO as opposed to Tinker Toy. Because the children who grow up with LEGO can't understand how to make something strong by making a triangle. So I sort of had the conjecture that although those people could build all sorts of wonderful houses and things, they ended up deficient in having the most important of all architectural concepts. A triangle is infinitely strong, because you can't alter a triangle without breaking it, whereas, I don't know what. That's a run-on sentence I can't finish. AUDIENCE: This explains the deterioration of society, Marvin. We don't have Tinker Toys and we don't have chemistry sets with chemicals that make explosives anymore. [LAUGHTER] You have to go to terrorist school to get a good education. [LAUGHTER] AUDIENCE: So actually in this conversation about being good at things and learning how to learn better, I think that a point that sort of relates to this idea of Tinker sets and playing around with things, I think that it's not enough to simply come up with the best representations of a concept. In order to actually be good at something, whether it's music or speaking a new language, you have to not only understand it conceptually, but you actually have to gain a certain amount of fluency. And to gain fluency, you do have to play around with the thing a lot, whether it's turning it around in your mind or practicing it physically. So in the case of math, it's like, yeah, you can come up with all these different representations of it. And that's the first step, understanding it. And it's great once you understand it. But just because you understand the concept, like on a conceptual level, doesn't mean that you can actually know when to use it or know how to use it when you're solving a problem. And similarly, for music-- I guess I'm mostly talking in the case of improvisational music when I'm trying to speak something with the music. So I have something that I want to say. And maybe it's something that sort of low level. I'm trying to resolve one chord to get to some sort of cadence. Now, I can have multiple ways of resolving the chord. And in order to do this, I have a vocabulary of the different ways. And if one way doesn't occur to me when I'm playing the piece, I can try another way. But the important thing is that I have some way that I can resolve it in real-time, or else my piece is never going to come out. And then same thing about learning different languages or speaking different languages. In order to be able to speak or to express ourselves, we have to have not only understanding of the language, of the structure, but the immediacy of being able to access it. And that comes with practice, with fingering, what have you. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, that goes in several directions. Where in our educational system do we-- in grade school, is there a place where you emphasize having several representations? Because I can't think clearly right now. But it seems to me that you're usually trying to tell them the one best way. AUDIENCE: It's like, A, there's the idea of the one best way, and B, there's the idea of reinforcing the same process over and over again. So when you learn math, it's like, you learn this technique, and you reinforce the technique by doing a bunch of homework problems that are essentially like repetitions of the same thing. Whereas, I think a better way of doing it-- well, two better ways-- A, you have multiple representations. And 2, you create problems where you make people traverse paths differently. And different people may have different solutions. And each time you solve the problem you may have a different solution. But the idea is, you lay out a whole network of paths in your head to solve any given type of problem. MARVIN MINSKY: OK, so where in grade school do you ask children to solve the same problem three ways? Can anybody think of-- is that part of education. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] fractions. MARVIN MINSKY: What? AUDIENCE: That's the the closest thing I think of-- fractions. AUDIENCE: What about literature class, where they ask you for interpretations of novels. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes. I bet there are things that happen in literature that don't happen anywhere else in the curriculum. But most children don't transfer it. AUDIENCE: Another thing, in China, in learning math, when we try to find areas of certain geometric shapes, we always do it multiple times, multiple ways. MARVIN MINSKY: In topology, whatever it is, you just make it into triangles and simplexes. [LAUGHTER] So that's a very strange subject. AUDIENCE: Maybe [INAUDIBLE] so nice is because we can think about it as logical concepts, like [INAUDIBLE] sets, [INAUDIBLE] points, stuff like that. And then you [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Co-sets. Where in real life do you have duality? That's a nice feature of a lot of mathematics. Whatever you're doing in some fields, there's a dual way, where you look at the space of the functions on the objects rather than the objects. Where is that in-- is there anything like that in real life? Because in mathematics, a lot of problems suddenly become much easier in their dual form. It would just change everything. AUDIENCE: There's a question, Marvin. MARVIN MINSKY: I've been facing one way. AUDIENCE: I guess a couple of points-- you said, why is it difficult? Whenever I've struggled, I think it's because it's constructive, and you have to code a lot in your head. You have to code the entire structure of [INAUDIBLE] field. Because if you're learning algebra topology, you're holding all of algebra and all of that structure in your head. And so sometimes it becomes difficult if you have it constructed at the right level. And what I found, I think, my advisor was just really good. Many times, he basically said two things. Really good mathematicians are really good at making analogies in mathematics. And [INAUDIBLE] geometry. And he says, and really good algebraic geometers can boil everything down to linear algebra. And he said you can only do that if you abstract at the right level. And he never gave techniques of doing that. But I think the difficulty with that [INAUDIBLE].. AUDIENCE: An analogy is the relationship between two objects. And if you're good at making analogies, you're at a level beyond, a level above just looking at objects. You're looking at relationships of objects. And regarding the practicing thing, I mean, it's still related to representations, because at each practice you're learning something new about these type of problems that might make you better at identifying them in the future. And it's not like numerous practices. The number of practices doesn't matter to your ability of solving problems. It's like, what you learn from each practice, if you can do a thing once-- I have a friend who basically told me how to do math. He's like, you look at a problem, solve it once, you go back and you think about how you solved it, like what's the process you used to solve it. MARVIN MINSKY: So a good problem is, make up another problem like this. AUDIENCE: That's essentially what you are learning when you are practicing. MARVIN MINSKY: It's probably too hard to great. You can't teach things you can't grade in the modern-- Yes. AUDIENCE: So I believe that math is too abstract. And so it's difficult to go from one to representation to another, and that would be the whole problem. I can't learn a new concept without having a concept that's very near that concept, that's very similar. So it's just that, if I don't have good representations of a lot of things, it's difficult to continue representation. So when I learned, I don't know, topology, I should know analogies. And then I can go from there to there, because the representation is very close. And so people that are good at math, maybe they have a lot of representations so it's easier to add a new representation of a thing, because it's close. MARVIN MINSKY: It certainly would be nice to know. AUDIENCE: Like in the example of vectors, I already have the concept of, I don't know, the perpendicular lines. And so just adding more lines it's easy. But I don't know, the n dimensional thing, it's very abstract. I don't have any other representation that's close by that concept. It's just that I need a lot of concepts and representations. And I need one that's close by. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, between the vectors and the Fourier, they're so different. What would be in between those two? AUDIENCE: The Fourier is the [INAUDIBLE] kind of concepts. MARVIN MINSKY: Actually square waves-- probably square waves are easier to understand than sines and cosines. But they're not continuous-- I mean, not differential. Who has a problem to solve? AUDIENCE: I can comment on, I think, [INAUDIBLE] comment on having lots of practice. And I don't think that actually is so much out of the representational view. I don't know what's going on neurologically. But if you have new representations, if you assume that they are symbolic representations, in this sense, they are quite generative things. You can combine them and you can make a lot of stuff. But usually when you will learn something new, like if you learn the rule, there's going to be exceptions. So when you repeat things, one of the reasons for that might be to find that box. You might not [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Well now, when you practice a little piece of music by repeating, do you change your representation or do you just repeat and hope it gets better? AUDIENCE: So I think there's a difference between-- OK, so there's the practice in this sense of traditional classical music practice. And then there's the idea of tinker practice, if you will. So I think that the type of practice that I'm advocating is the type of practice where you're actually sort of turning around the concept or the thing, the object in your head, so that you're looking at it from many different perspectives and connecting it to many different means, which is actually conducive to expanding your representation, connecting it to different concepts, like all the good things that help us remember it and better use it. This is very different from the classical music practice. Having been a classical pianist for 18 years, it's not a really good way of doing things. AUDIENCE: I was wondering, has there been any studies of children, or have there been any children who have just played piano or some keyboard instrument [INAUDIBLE] over their lives, and then they suddenly have something where, when you press it, where you can control the volume? MARVIN MINSKY: A theremin. AUDIENCE: Yeah, yeah, yeah. But if a child very suddenly could learn a completely new dimension, that's the dynamics and how would it react to that. So I don't know if there are any such cases. But I would suspect that it would go to [INAUDIBLE] backward [INAUDIBLE] and then find some children [INAUDIBLE].. MARVIN MINSKY: There was a nice period in the Media Lab when we were building three-dimensional theremin for Penn of Penn and Teller. He's quite a good musician. We were making gadgets so he could wave his hands. But I wonder, in classical, there ought to be some very short pieces that come in 10 variations. Because we make children learn fairly long pieces where it's just repeating. AUDIENCE: There's the Diabelli Variations, which is like 32 or 33 really short pieces. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, but only eight people in the world can play it. AUDIENCE: But I guess, the thing with classical music is that, it kind of just makes you learn this one thing. And you learn it by repeating it over and over again. Whereas in something like jazz that's more improvisational, it's like you have a template. And each time you go through it, you can traverse a different path through it. But even more like classical, classical music, where you're playing the same thing each time, I think that there's still like a good way of doing it and a not so good way, the good way being, each time you practice it, you subtly vary it somehow. Like you change the expression of it, you play it faster or slower, things like that. And I guess this goes back to the whole-- it's like, each time you reinforce an idea, like simply repeating it, well, in the beginning, it might help you familiarize yourself with the idea. But if you repeat it and vary it slightly each time to look at different dimensions of it, then you learn it better. MARVIN MINSKY: How many of you know that piece, Beethoven's Diabelli? Well, you should google it up and listen to it. It has 32. Is it 32? AUDIENCE: I think it's 32, and then there's the original theme. So it's like 33 [INAUDIBLE] maybe. MARVIN MINSKY: The one I like is the next to last, which is [WORDLESS SINGING]. AUDIENCE: Oh, the fugue, yeah. MARVIN MINSKY: The fugue. So that'll give you another view of classical music, because the pieces are fairly short and they all have some ideas in common. And it's sort of like poetry. What do you call those poems where there are many verses and each verse ends with the same line, but it means something different each time? AUDIENCE: What's an example? MARVIN MINSKY: What? AUDIENCE: What's an example of it? Can you think of a poem? MARVIN MINSKY: I couldn't hear you. AUDIENCE: Well, there's like the villanelle-- MARVIN MINSKY: It's like a rondo, except that it changes its meaning each time. And it's the same words. They're pretty hard to make, I guess. AUDIENCE: There's the villanelle, which has a bit of that. There's the famous one that's, what, like, do not go gentle into the night or something. Rage, rage against the coming of something. MARVIN MINSKY: Anyway, I'll email you the Diabelli. I have a friend, Manfred Clynes, who wrote this book called Sentics, who used to play that particular Beethoven thing. Well, last important question. Thanks for coming. [LAUGHTER]
Info
Channel: MIT OpenCourseWare
Views: 32,835
Rating: 4.8425198 out of 5
Keywords: consciousness, dennett
Id: oG6FyY2r9G0
Channel Id: undefined
Length: 109min 44sec (6584 seconds)
Published: Tue Mar 04 2014
Related Videos
Note
Please note that this website is currently a work in progress! Lots of interesting data and statistics to come.