2. Falling In Love

Video Statistics and Information

Video
Captions Word Cloud
Reddit Comments
Captions
The following content is provided under a Creative Commons license. Your support will help MIT OpenCourseWare continue to offer high-quality educational resources for free. To make a donation or to view additional materials from hundreds of MIT courses, visit MIT OpenCourseWare at ocw.mit.edu. MARVIN MINSKY: I usually start by asking if there are any questions. But I thought I'd say a few things about chapter 1, and then see if there are any questions. I can't see the pointer. Oh, anybody remember how to get Word to [CHUCKLES] make its pointer not disappear? Maybe I mentioned this the first lecture, but I was taken by this cute poem by Dorothy Parker because the first chapter was about love and stuff like that. So I tried to get the rights to reproduce it, and it turned out that she was angry at all her friends. She must have been a perpetually pissed-off person. And so she left all her literary rights to the NAACP. And I called them up for hours and they couldn't find the rights. So finally-- so it's in the version of the motion machine on the web. But I had to resort to Shakespeare to replace her. Shakespeare's a slightly better poet, but he's not as funny as Dorothy Parker. [CHUCKLES] So the first chapter starts out-- or, it's mostly about all the things we don't understand about the mind, which is almost everything. And the first discussion is-- well, the whole chapter is making fun of the most popular ideas. And the most popular idea of the mind is that people think that they are not doing the thinking, but there's something inside them that's doing the thinking. And it's this idea that there is a self-- is embedded in just about everything we say and think. And really, it's hard to see how you would do without it. But if you ask, what is the self? Then since this idea is so popular, people begin to believe that there is such a thing and it takes all sorts of various forms. And the most dangerous form, maybe, is the one that religions exploit. Which is that inside a person, with all their complications, there's a little pure essence called the soul, or the self, or whatever you want to call it, and it's impossible to describe it or explain it in physical terms. And so that is one of the reasons why we think there are two worlds-- a physical world and lots of other kinds of worlds. And each of us has some imaginary model of what they are and what they're in. And philosophers talk about it and existentialists, and so on. So there are lots of problems about our ideas about ourselves. And in reading around for half my life, I was puzzled at the strange ideas that are around. And in Aristotle, I find the first intelligible theories of mind and emotions. So if you look at, in particular, Aristotle's-- there are a number of books and one of them is called Rhetoric. And it's full of theories about how people reason and influence each other. And I'll show you some quotes from that because when I look at the history that I've encountered about psychology, sort of Aristotle stands out as being the first and among the best. And as far as I can see, there were no psychologists nearly as good as him. Of course, we don't know whether there was a him exactly because he-- what we have of Aristotle is a lot of writing, but it's all cobbled together by students from all sorts of manuscripts by other people, and people who took notes. And Aristotle claims to have learned a lot from Plato. Well, we have very little writing from him. And so there you go-- three centuries before the Christian era, as it's called. And then a couple of thousand years later, you start to find people like Spinoza and Kant and John Locke and David Hume, who start to make psychology theories, very little of which is as good as the ones that Aristotle has in all his fragments. So one question that frequently bothers me, and it should bother everyone, is why did science disappear for 1,000 years? And the standard explanation is the rise of the great religions. And why did it come back? And you see with the first signs of anything like modern science, at least in my view, with Galileo and Newton-- there a couple of people before that. There's some people in the Muslim world who invented some high school algebra, and they make a big fuss about that. It looks like Archimedes, in a very recently discovered manuscript, computed an integral. He found the volume of a cone, which is-- what is it? 1/6 bh-- I forget. Anyway, so why did science disappear and why did psychology appear so late? Because there isn't much psychology in the modern sense until 1900, or the late 1800s, with Francis Galton and William James lived around here. And Rudolph Fleiss, who-- Sigmund Freud starts writing in 1895. People make fun of Freud, as I mentioned last week. But in fact, among other things-- how many of you have read the recent criticisms of Freud, which claim that he was a complete faker and never cured a single patient? This is popular stuff. I don't believe Freud ever really claimed to cure a single patient. So the critics, who are really very ferocious, claimed that he made up all his data, and so forth. But most of what Freud says, is that psychoanalysis might be a good way to find out what you're really thinking, and discover more about yourself and your goals, and so forth. And I had the good or bad luck to be introduced to L. Ron Hubbard by John when I was an undergraduate. John Campbell was the great editor of the-- I think it was called Astounding Science Fiction in those days-- what a marvelous title. And this fairly mediocre science fiction writer, L. Ron Hubbard, invented a new form of psychiatry called-- what was it called? AUDIENCE: Dianetics. MARVIN MINSKY: Dianetics. AUDIENCE: Dianetics. MARVIN MINSKY: [CHUCKLES] It's pretty good. And I'll tell you that story another time. But John Campbell had Thanksgiving in the Commander Hotel every year and invited a bunch of friends. And I don't remember if that's how I got to meet Asimov and Heinlein, and other people. But anyway, I did, and science fiction had a big influence on me from my, actually, early years. But starting in college, it got very serious. Anyway, so chapter 1 starts to talk about this phenomenon of psychology. And one of the funny parts is this little section, 1.3, of trying to say, what are emotions? And I looked up emotions in dictionaries and-- can you all read that? I don't feel like reading aloud. But there's lots of discussion of emotions, and how mysterious and complex they are. And then the marvelous thing is how many words there are for emotional states. I think I got 300, but I don't remember. Anyway, here's from A to D, and I don't recall how I found those. But I think that's a lot. How many words for ways to think are there? Now that's a serious question because I complained, maybe, on the next page. No, I didn't. I found myself complaining that there were very few words for ways to think. And then this afternoon when I was pruning these slides, it occurred to me that I didn't really try. So maybe I just didn't think enough. So if there are a couple of words for everyday emotions, if any of you can find me a list of 10 or 20 common words for styles of thinking, I'd appreciate it. 'Cause I wonder if there are a lot, and if not, why not? So here is a list of typical situations-- grieving for a lost child, panic at being in an enclosed space. I'm not sure any of the words in the list of 300 standard emotions are good enough to describe how you feel for any of these not unusual states. "Have you ever lost control of your car at high "speed? No, but when I first learned to drive, I couldn't believe that you could read signs at the same time as-- well, anyway. One of the very best psychologists in history, or a pair of psychologists, aren't even called psychologists. These are two guys named Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen. And they-- somebody made up the word ethology. What they study is the behavior of animals. And in some sense, presumably, they're studying the psychology of animals because just as with the person, when somebody flies into a rage, you're not describing their mental state. You're describing something about how they behave. And so the ethologists, too, are psychologists. And Tinbergen and Lorenz, starting around 1920s, started to analyze the behavior of animals in great detail. And so here's an example of how a certain fish behaves. I actually forgot which fish it is, but there's a picture of it some-- in the book. And at different points in its life, it's in different phases. And this is just one diagram of a dozen for this particular fish and its reproduction, which involves an environment with plants and other things. And he divides its behavior into parenting, courtship, nesting, and fighting. And then, you see each of those as a lot of subdivisions. And Tinbergen and Lorenz and some students discovered all these things by sitting in front of fish tanks and watching the fish for months and years. Tinbergen also spent years on some beach watching seagulls, and so he has a diagram like this for a particular class of seagulls. When I came to Boston, my friends and I used to go to Nahant and look at the tide pools there, where there are a lot of activities, and it was very interesting. And I got a big fish tank and imported all sorts of little animals and plants from the tide pools in Nahant. And I watched them for about a year and didn't learn anything. AUDIENCE: [LAUGHS] MARVIN MINSKY: And that was before I read Tinbergen. And then I realized there's something about those people, which is they could watch a fish and recognize all sorts of behaviors. And I would just watch a fish and wonder whether it was hungry, or-- what? Wouldn't you get bored swimming back and forth for three years in a-- anyway. So here, the great psychologists of our day-- Aristotle, 2,000 years ago, and Lorenz and Tinbergen in the 1930s, and Sigmund Freud and Galton and William James around 1900. And then what went wrong? There's almost no good psychology between then and 1950s, when something called cognitive psychology started. And it was partly due to people who said, let's make psychology more scientific. And you've probably all heard of Pavlov or Watson. And what happened is, around 1900, some psychologists said, well, these Galtons and Freuds and William James are very poetic and expressive and literary, and they write much better than we do and tells good stories. But they're not scientists and they don't do reproducible experiments. So what we have to do is simplify the situation to find the basic laws of behavior. So let's take a pigeon and put it in a vacuum in the dark. Well-- [CHUCKLES] AUDIENCE: [LAUGHS] MARVIN MINSKY: Well, they didn't go that far. But they did put it in the dark and there were two illuminated levers to work. And you could make a sound and the sound could be very annoying, or you could have a bright annoying flashing light, or something. And the animal would push one of two levers and one of them would make the stimulus even more annoying and one of them would make it go away. And you'd plot curves of how often the animal pressed these levers so you would get a quantitative theory of how much he learned and how much it could remember, and how many trials it would have to do. And then instead of just looking at reactions to stimuli, they quickly switched to trying to teach the animal things by giving it to two alternatives-- turn left or turn right, or push this button or that, or whatever. And if they pushed the one you approve of, then you'd give them a little pellet of food. And there was a lot of engineering so that you would make sure that the food got to them right away. Because if there were a 10-second delay between an action and a reward, the pigeon or a squirrel or rat or cat or dog would learn much less quickly than if there was a one-second delay. And anyway, that went on for 50 years, starting around 1900, Pavlov and his dogs. And there's a great movie that some guy came around with that had been taken of Pavlov's lab, and it shows him sort of like a great dictator, or something. There's this room with lot of cages and dogs, and mostly dogs, in this case. And Pavlov comes in and there are a bunch of lackeys who sort of bow and scrape because he's a lord. And he comes in, and all the dogs run into the corner of their cages and yelp. So the Pavlovians tried pretty hard to get that movie suppressed. And I haven't seen it in recent years. But anyway, Fred Skinner, who was a professor when I was an undergraduate at Harvard, was the first one to really automate this experimental psychology, and he invented what's called a Skinner box. But it's just a soundproof, light-proof, well-ventilated and thermally-regulated cage. And you can put a rat or a pigeon-- those are the most common animals. They're very inexpensive because they're free. No one knows much about dolphins. They've been studied for 50 years. Whenever John what's-his-name-- remember the name of the great dolphin-- AUDIENCE: Lilly. MARVIN MINSKY: Lilly-- thanks. He discovered a lot about dolphins and a certain amount about their communication, and a little bit about whales. But there's an interesting mystery-- I forget which whales, but some whales have a 20-minute song, and they repeat it for a whole season. And next year, that song is a little bit different. But it goes, essentially, without repeating-- it's very complicated-- for 20 minutes. And people have studied that a lot and no one has the slightest idea of what it means. And nobody even has any good conjectures, which bothers me. But I think it probably means is this, when there's a whole bunch of fish somewhere for one of these whales, it might be 200 miles away, and it's very-- whales eat a lot. And it's very important to find where the fish are. And I believe this message, which changes a bit during the season, might be telling you where the food is on the Atlantic or Pacific coastline in great detail. Because if somebody finds a lot of fish somewhere, you have to swim 300 miles. And if they're not there-- so anyway, it's interesting that John Lilly got a lot of publicity, but he didn't discover squat. And finally, the dolphin studiers gave up because nothing happened. Anybody have heard anything in-- I haven't paid any attention for 20 years. Have you heard of anybody discovering anything about dolphins except they're very good at solving a lot of physical problems? Anyway, that's-- I'm bothered by the mystery of why was there some psychology in Aristotle's time? And why didn't it get anywhere till 1950, when there was regular psychology, but it was afflicted by what I call, physics envy. Namely, you run into people like Estes and-- well, he was pretty good, actually. But there a lot of psychologists who made up things, like Maxwell's equations for how animals learn, and there were generally three or four laws. And if there's a sequence of events, then animals remember a lot about the first few and the last few in the sequence. They don't remember much about the middle. And of course, the reliability of their memory depends a lot on how recent it was and on how powerful the reward was, and blah, blah. And so they get these little sets of rules that look like Newton's laws, and that was the kind of sort of psychological physics that the so-called behaviorists were mostly looking for. This was not what Tinbergen and Lorenz did because they wrote books with extensive descriptions of what the animals did and made little diagrammatic guesses about the structure of the subroutines and sub-structures. Anyway, end of history, but it's a nice question-- why do some sciences grow, and why was psychology just about the last one? I suspect it could have been earlier, but people tried to imitate the physicists and tried to say, maybe there's something like Newton's or Maxwell's laws for the mind. And they found a few, but they weren't enough to explain much. So there are a lot of questions. When Seymour Papert and I started thinking about these things, which was really around 1960, I had been working on some ideas about AI in the late 50s. And my PhD thesis was a theory of neural networks, which was sort of interesting, but never really went anywhere. I went to a meeting in London somewhere and gave a talk about a theory of learning that was based on some neural network ideas. And there was this person from South Africa named Seymour Papert who gave the same paper. And I hope this happens to you someday. Find somebody who thinks so much like you, only different enough that it's worth it. And that you only have to say about three words a day, and some whole new thing starts. Because we really did write the same paper and it had the same equation in it. And he had been working for Piaget, who was the first great child psychologist. I should have mentioned Piaget, who probably discovered more things about psychology than any other single person in history. And there are lots of people now who say he was wrong about point 73 because children learn that at the age of 2 and 1/2 instead of 2 and 3/4. I'm parodying the Piaget critic community, but it's pretty bad. And I think those poor guys are uncomfortable because John Piaget published 20 books full of observations about children that, as far as I know, no one had made systematically before. And in his later years, he started courting algebraic mathematicians because he wanted more formal theories. And in my view, he wanted to make his theories worse. And nothing much happened, but he did visit here a couple of times and it was really exciting to meet the starter of a whole new field. Anyway, Papert and I discussed lots of things. And somehow or other, we kept finding other-- more ideas about psychology. And it finally gelled into the idea that, well, if you look at the brain, you know that there are several different brain centers. What's all that stuff for, and how could it possibly make any sense to try to explain what it does in terms of four laws like Newton? Like, how does a car work? Is there a magical force inside the engine that causes the wheels to turn? No. There's this funny thing in the back to cause a differential so that if the car isn't going in a straight line, the two wheels going at different speeds won't rip the-- if the two wheels were going at the same speed, the tread would come off in 5 minutes. You ever wonder what a differential is for? Well. So most of the car is fixing bugs in the other parts. That's most of the brain is because we started out as fish, or lizards, or whatever you like. And making the brain bigger wouldn't help much because you just get a heavier lizard that had to eat more and would think more slowly. So size is bad. But on the other hand, if you need another cubic inch of brain to fix the bugs in the other part, then the evolutionary advantage of being smarter had better make you able to catch a little more food per hour. And so each person is an ecology of these different processes. And the brain reached its present size about a million years ago, I guess. What's the current guess? Anybody been track-- they keep discovering new ancestors of humans and I don't have the patience to read about them because you know that next week, somebody will say, oh, that isn't in the main line and you were just unlucky to discover that skeleton. So anyway, Papert and I and a lot of students gradually developed this picture that the mind is made of lots of processes, or agents, or resources, or whatever you want to call them. And it's anybody's guess what they are. If you look at the anatomy of the brain, you know that people label regions, so it's very clear that this occipital lobe back here is largely concerned with vision. But I forget where the one for hearing is. If you destroy the part of the brain for hearing in some animals, you get a little bit of increased function in some part of the visual system that seems to enable the animal to hear a little bit and make some reactions. And there's a whole lot of hype-- I think you have to call it-- about the flexibility of the nervous system. That is, if certain brain areas get destroyed, other parts take over. They almost never are as good. And mostly, many functions never get taken over at all, but are replaced by ones that superficially seem similar. And so there's a whole lot of, I guess, wishful thinking that the brain is immensely resourceful and error-correcting and repairing. I think there was some idea that there was a general phenomenon. But if you do some arithmetic, you get an interesting result. Suppose that each function in the brain occurred in 10 different places at random. Then, if you removed half the brain, how many functions would you lose? Well, almost no arithmetic tells you you would lose about one part in the 1,000. And so, in fact, you would never be able to detect it. So this idea that the brain has enormous redundancy-- well, now change that number to five. Suppose each function is somewhat supported in five different parts of the brain. Then if you take off half the brain, then-- what am I saying? One part in 32 chance of losing some significant function, so probably lots of things that we do are supported in several parts of the brain. Apparently, the language center is pretty unique, and some others. But be careful about the conclusions you read from optimistic neuroscientists. Anyway, Papert and I worked on this idea of how could these large numbers of different processes be organized? And we made various theories about it. And then around, I guess, the late 1970s, we stopped working together. And Papert developed his revolutionary ideas about education, which, certainly, have had a lot of influence, although they didn't sweep the world in the way we had hoped. And I kept working on the society of mind theory, and we didn't work together so much. But we still did plenty of criticizing and supporting of each other. Anyway, my theory ended up with this idea that-- it's sort of based on Freud. I don't know if I kept a picture of his here. Freud concluded that the mind was an interesting arena, sort of. And he had the mind divided into three parts. At one end of the mind, which we inherited from most other animals, is called the id, which is a bunch of instinctive, mainly built-in behavioral mechanisms. And a second part of the mind is what he called the super ego, which is a collection of critics. So in Freud's first image, the brain is in two parts. One is a set of instinctive, built-in behaviors and the other is a set of critics, which actually are associated with a culture or-- well, with culture and a tradition. And you learn from other people things that are good to do and things that are bad to do, and that's called the super ego. This is your set of values and standards and tests for suitable behavior. And the middle is this strange object called the ego, which is not what people think it is-- at least Freud's word. The ego is a kind of big neutral battleground where the instinctive behaviors-- I keep wanting to-- oh, you can see that arrow if I take my finger off it. And then gradually, as I kept trying to figure out how problems are solved and what kind of processes might be involved, I got this picture, which has six layers. And various people come around and say, I don't think you need to distinguish between layers four and five, or why don't you just lump all the three top layers into one? And I sort of laugh quietly and say, these people are trying to find a physics-like unified minimal theory of psychology. And they're probably right, in one sense. But if they do that, they'll get stuck. Because if you get a new idea, there'll be no place to put it. So if you have something that's very mysterious, don't imitate the physicists. Because if you make a theory that's exactly right and just accounts for the data, and there's nothing extra and nothing loose, then when you notice a new phenomenon, like dark matter, then the physicists don't know what to do. Should they regard dark matter as some obscure feature of space-time, or does it have something to do with this universe being near another one that you can't otherwise communicate with? And it's all very puzzling. But there are lots of things that don't fit into Newton's laws these days. And I'm not suggesting a six-layer theory of physics, but it might be worth a try. OK. So what am I-- I made up some nice slides. But I think I'll stop. So who has some questions, and what would you like to see in a theory of psychology? What do you want to be explained? A lot of people are convinced that there are some really serious problems and mysteries, like what is consciousness? And if you look at chapter 4, my feeling is consciousness is an etymological accident, that people got a word which is a suitcase for all of the things they don't understand about the mind and more. But once you've got a word and it goes in the culture-- consider the word consciousness for a minute from a legal point of view. Supposing you happened to be walking along and you're carrying something-- where is that pointer-- and it happens to stick somebody's eye out. Then it's very important when they sue you to establish whether you meant to do it, or whether it was an accident. Did you consciously plan to-- I can't think of the English word for putting somebody's eye out. There's beheading and all sorts of-- AUDIENCE: Gouge? MARVIN MINSKY: Gouge is a good word. AUDIENCE: Impale? MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah. So anyway, it's very important, for social reasons, to have a word for whether an action was deliberately violating the rules, as opposed to accidentally violating the rules. Like if you tripped on the stairs and landed on somebody and broke their neck, that's not a crime unless you were so clever as to make it appear that it was an accident, and probably-- anyway, you see what I mean. So we need a-- our whole system of fairness and ethics and social responsibility is based on the distinction between whether an action was deliberate or not. And so, did he do that consciously-- is a word for that. And somehow, the idea of conscious became elevated. Well, that's a very superficial-- you can probably think of 10 other reasons why a word like that-- yes? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] in terms of your writing, I-- it seems to be that there is both sides of the argument, both sides of it. But [INAUDIBLE] it seems [INAUDIBLE] in that-- I can well imagine is lack of representation. You have representation of self and representation of your mind. But then you say, there's no self or no consciousness. Why can't you think about consciousness as just a process that is reasoning about your own mind? MARVIN MINSKY: Well-- AUDIENCE: Is there a problem with that? I mean, I understand you don't want to talk about-- MARVIN MINSKY: No. AUDIENCE: --soul, 'cause that's a religious notion. But-- MARVIN MINSKY: No, but sometimes, when you say conscious, that is, do you remember doing it, which is-- AUDIENCE: Yeah, but I don't I load up my process with-- asks me what I think about my own mind? And then I retrieve that and I say, yeah, I do remember it. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, you're right, actually. I went to a lot of trouble to find 25 or 30 different uses for the word consciousness. And probably, if I-- or if one of us worked harder, we could take those and condense them into five or six much better ones that account for more stuff than the 30. AUDIENCE: Well, I think you got five right there-- ideas, goals, memory, thoughts, and feelings, so-- MARVIN MINSKY: That might be just right for something. Who knows? [CHUCKLES] Yes, well, I think that's a great criticism of one reason why people don't like these theories quite so much, because I proposed too many things. I really should reprint that criticism from Restak, or hand it out, because it's this neurologist who says, why is he telling us all these things about K-lines and representations, and so forth? And the answer is, he's from the community that doesn't have enough variety yet. And I'd be the first to admit that I try to go overboard and think of five more things than are in the literature. But that emotion thing is nice. Remember that was a serious challenge because when I made that list-- [CHUCKLES] I do have a laser pointer somewhere-- in my jacket, probably. How many words for at least noticeably distinct ways of thinking, or reasoning, or figuring out, or solving problems can you think of? Maybe there are 20 or 30. I just realized this afternoon that I never looked. I don't remember where I got this list. Yeah? AUDIENCE: Can I ask about your perception of free will? And I gather from the readings that you don't have a strong sense of free will, and so what is that-- MARVIN MINSKY: I think it's the same as the one for consciousness-- namely, it's a legal concept. The idea of free will is completely obscene, isn't it? What could it possibly mean if you did something for no reason? So it's a thoroughly empty idea, isn't it? Or what do you mean by it? Do you mean there's nobody ordering you around, so you're free to do whatever you want? But of course, you're not. You can only do what your computer computes you to do. AUDIENCE: [LAUGHS] AUDIENCE: So in a sense, in the same way that you draw the-- you showed the fish diagram, right? The fish's actions are a product of the environment in its current state, and it's essentially a Turing machine-- MARVIN MINSKY: Well, it's some kind of machine, yes, sure. AUDIENCE: And so, would you argue that we are also Turing machines that are just running out in the world and-- MARVIN MINSKY: Sure. I've never heard of any even interesting alternative. In other words, people who insist on free will appear to me to be like people who believe that there must be a god who created the world. What's the next step? Who created the god? They don't take that step. So if your will is free, OK, then who is controlling it? There's nothing there. But legally, it's great. Because if somebody stole so money of their own free will-- but suppose you were a peculiar kind of epileptic. And every now then, when you go by-- AUDIENCE: [LAUGHS] MARVIN MINSKY: --your hand goes out and steals things? Then they-- what do they do? They put you on parole? No, this is very strange. But if you look at religions, you see that people make money on them. 13% of the world's product goes to people who make their living on concepts like free will and consciousness, so it's a big money thing. It's not just an accident. It's an industry. AUDIENCE: So both of those are concepts of society. MARVIN MINSKY: They're [INAUDIBLE] AUDIENCE: You could imagine a society without the concept of consciousness and free will, but those are [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: I don't think you could. You'd have to make up something to keep people in check and under control and to train them. It's like the rat being-- the rat needs somebody to press the reward or punish button. And we have it built into our-- our culture works because you build into people's head the machinery for suppressing doubt. And it's very clever. But you should think of it as an industry rather than an inexplicable phenomenon. How much money goes into-- yes? AUDIENCE: How many ways of thinking can you think of? MARVIN MINSKY: Say it again? AUDIENCE: How many ways of thinking can you think of right now? MARVIN MINSKY: That's my challenge, in fact. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: There's probably a big list in some chapter or other. But there's nothing like this. What's the trick? Three-- if I go like that. Actually, Dragon has a thing so you can tell it, make things bigger. How many of you use the new Dragon program? Speech thing. I can't believe how good it is. Oh, I was talking to Henry Lieberman about it earlier. AUDIENCE: So I'm under the impression that you would say that the [INAUDIBLE] [? of mind ?] theory applies both to humans and animals. Is it just that we have higher levels of organization than them? And so where would you draw that line? Do animals not have a notion of the self the way you described in the book? Like long-term planning and stuff? MARVIN MINSKY: That's a great question. And it'd be interesting to think about ways to investigate it. People or researchers are often, in fact, raising that question of do animals have a representation of themself? And there's a famous experiment, but I can't remember what its current status is, where you put a red dot on a chimpanzee's head. And when the chimp passes a mirror and sees that, the chimp might go like that. Whereas I don't think a dog, when it passes a mirror, would rub its forehead to see if it has a red spot. I had a cat who walked past mirrors, because we have some full-sized mirrors around the house. And the cat walks by, and there's this other cat in the mirror, and she pays no attention to it whatever. So of course, I don't know what happened the first three times she walked by that mirror. Because if you see another cat going by, you would think it would-- anyway, it's a good question. And people ask that. And there's some evidence that elephants have a model of themself and maybe dolphins. Have any of you heard any stories of other animals that can recognize, for example, when they've been painted? AUDIENCE: Elephants? MARVIN MINSKY: Which? AUDIENCE: You said elephants? MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, I think elephants. AUDIENCE: Infants can't [INAUDIBLE] There's a famous child psychology experiment, where if you're less than a couple months old, you actually fail this test. MARVIN MINSKY: Oh. AUDIENCE: So it's actually something that comes as a sign of your child progressing. So it might not be intrinsic to humanity [INAUDIBLE] So [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: This is off the topic, but I once had a great email correspondence with some woman who was getting a PhD in France about how babies recognize their mothers. And she concluded with-- she did experiments, having other people walk into the room with a mask of the mother or a different hairdo and so forth. And for the first two months or three months, I think, it turned out that the baby recognizes the mother by the hairdo, which had not been known. And then, I think after three months, it's recognizing the mother by her face. And that point, she's doing experiments where you get another woman wearing a copy of the mother's face. And so now there's two mothers, and the baby is absolutely delighted. And then I can't remember, but then I think at four or five months, when two copies of the mother comes in, the baby gets really panicked. So I lost track of her. If you have a baby, let me know. She got her PhD for this. And I haven't heard anything since. Yes? AUDIENCE: Here's a half-formulated thought that I just thought of regarding ways of thinking and ways of feeling. So it just occurred to me that it seems like if you go back to the list of feelings, it seems like when we talk about feeling, we're talking about a state that the brain is in. So it might be a complicated state that's like some combination of a lot of different parameters. But it's a state that you can stay in for like an arbitrary amount of time. But I think thinking is something that's more sequential, as in like when you're thinking, you're necessarily changing the state of your brain all the time because you're moving bits around. So I don't know. I just thought of this. MARVIN MINSKY: No, I think that's right. That I think when we talk about intelligent behavior, you're absolutely right. What you've got is a process that's criticizing itself and seeing when you got stuck and finding things to do. And I suppose in each emotional state, you're certainly also thinking. So that's going on, but maybe it's more restricted. Like if you're confronting somebody, and there's a sort of conflict, then almost all your thoughts are constrained to that subject. And it's not as resourceful. But I'm just improvising. AUDIENCE: Or if you have for each emotional state, when we say that we're in an emotional state, does it mean that we're talking about are the certain switches that get flipped in a certain state, or something is above a certain threshold. And you can have thoughts that are about anything in that state. But perhaps the state itself influences the type of thoughts that you're likely to have. MARVIN MINSKY: OK. I think what I'm talking about in this context is sort of extreme forms, where the person changes into another machine. Like, an angry person won't listen to reason, or it's very hard to deflect them, and so it's this kind of rigid thing. But humans are generally-- are rarely in such extreme states, where nothing gets through. But the whole point of that was that-- I just realize that I maybe was just too lazy-- that we have this huge vocabulary of nuances of emotional activity. Also, people think that these are hard to explain and mysterious and non-physical and blah, blah, blah. But why don't we wait until next week and see if somebody comes up with-- or see if we can come up with a set of 30 or 40 words about intellectual states? Curiosity-- I just don't know how many there are. And I haven't thought of any in the last few minutes. Yes? Has anybody thought of a couple of-- AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] like Csikszentmihalyi's concept. MARVIN MINSKY: What's the word? AUDIENCE: Flow. AUDIENCE: Flow. AUDIENCE: Like Csikszentmihalyi, like when you're really engaged in some activity that you're doing, and you're really in the zone. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, there's a state of keeping other things out, so you can focus-- not being interruptable. Yes? AUDIENCE: So in this state of mind, you talk about agents and how they divide between themselves. But I don't see anywhere about like how evolution modified a lot agents. I believe that evolution modified the way we think right now. I don't know if you will solve-- there is a paper wrote like two years ago. It's called "The Region of Behavior." And this guy tried to explain how we make some decisions-- evolutionary, like in a point of view of our [? species. ?] We are maximizing the probability of really reproducing a self. But individually, we are not increasing this [? efficiency. ?] And I think somehow these agents will be the decisions of agents or resources would be very determined by evolution since we have a very long time to of evolution of the human being. And somehow, we have hardwired to make some decisions. So for example, he gives the example of a guy-- MARVIN MINSKY: Well, for lizards, that's certainly true. But why do [INAUDIBLE]---- humans keep changing their environment. AUDIENCE: So for example, yeah, so he uses this example of tossing a coin. The guy says that this coin is unfair. He doesn't say the probabilities. But there is 75% of getting heads, and 25% of getting tails. And we-- like the subjects-- they choose randomly 25% of the time the tails, even though they can take a count of the number of the times that you put heads. And even though if you choose always heads, you will get more money or whatever, you would make it the right decision. MARVIN MINSKY: That's called probability matching, and it's not a good strategy. AUDIENCE: Yeah, but if humans do that, like-- MARVIN MINSKY: No-- well, they do it if a psychologist rigs the experiment very carefully. It turns out that the best thing to-- what do you think is the optimal strategy? AUDIENCE: Always choose-- MARVIN MINSKY: No. The optimal strategy turns out it's the square roots of the probabilities normalized to add up to 1. And I'll give you a proof next time. This theorem is due to Ray Solomonoff, who invented inductive probability theory. AUDIENCE: But evolution-- MARVIN MINSKY: So evolution, if evolution did probability matching, then it would be wrong. And I bet you'll find out that those experiments are wrong. You have to see how did he rig the experiment so that people, if it's probability 25%, they guess that 25%? AUDIENCE: They made the experiments with [? goldfish. ?] MARVIN MINSKY: I don't know. There was a theory about why you would expect it. It's a good question. I don't think people use probabilities, though. So even if an experiment shows some, I would look for a flaw in the design of the experiment. Yes? AUDIENCE: So Professor Minsky, you talked a lot about emotions in today's lecture. And my understanding is that like for someone who has this distinct personality, they might have a predisposition to feel a certain emotion of like anger, depression, or whatnot. My question is that if you had any insight or theories on to what extent our personality is affected by events or influences that happen to us over the course of a lifetime. And to what extent is it impacted by chemical or biological makeup of our brain? MARVIN MINSKY: Well, you're asking what do people learn? We don't care if it's chemical or-- see, if it's chemical, it's still physical. AUDIENCE: I've read a little bit about treatments for depression. And the argument is that a lot of the reasons for depression is because of some sort of chemical and biological way our brain is constructed. MARVIN MINSKY: Well, there's lots of complicated things about the brain. One feature of the brain, that I don't know if everybody-- you know that there are inhibitory and excitatory synapses. When one neuron connects to another, the impulse that goes along the axon to the target neuron may reduce the probability or the strength of its firing or increase it. So that's called inhibiting or-- there's no-- or exciting. That's not quite the right word, is it? Now, generally in the nervous system, as a rule, but not always, if you follow a chain of activity, it goes inhibiting, exciting, inhibiting, exciting. If you have too many excitatory things, and there was a loop, then it would explode. And it would wear itself out in jig time. So there is this general feature of the anatomy that you alternate. So when somebody talks about a drug having an inhibitory effect, that's sort of weird because it's inhibiting half the neurons. And therefore, it's lowering the thresholds of the ones they're connected to and so on. I think the best thing is until you have a diagram of the functional relations between different brain centers, it might be best not to try to make generalizations about how the chemistry works. It's easy, but people think of adrenaline as a stimulant-- epinephrine. But in the nervous system, locally, it may be inhibiting things that are inhibiting something else. And so it appears to be exciting. Yes? AUDIENCE: So I have a problem understanding the difference between thoughts and emotions. I know it might be a simple thing. But the only thing that I can separate in my mind is that thoughts, let's call it a time constant, I can change it kind of [INAUDIBLE] Emotions-- the time constant is smaller. And I can control it again much less. But since there is no-- and at least in this class, there is no free will, how can I make a decision [INAUDIBLE] by my [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, I think it's a waste of time. As far as I can see, emotional mechanisms are generally lower-level, simpler ones than the ones that involve several layers of the-- more layers of the brain. So it's just a relative thing. It's not that some states are emotional. You're always having some high-level thoughts and low-level thoughts. And the distinction-- I just don't know why the distinction has occupied so much tension. I think it's because-- and that goes back to having more words for-- or asking how many words do we have for ways to think. It seems to me that in popular culture, there are very few words for ways to think and lots of words for emotions, and so they are very prominent. Maybe you have to be smarter to distinguish between ways to think, and people generally are dumb-- not because they're inherently dumb, but they come from cultures which bully you if you-- what happens if you're in third grade, and you're smart? You get it beaten out of you, and you learn not to show it. AUDIENCE: I just think that question of why didn't science happen earlier? Why don't we have more ways to describe different ways to think? So that's sort of puzzling. But have we just not reflected as much on different thinking states or different approaches? MARVIN MINSKY: I wonder if the Greeks had more when we-- AUDIENCE: I think they did. I think they had also more concept of ideas in different states and different potencies. MARVIN MINSKY: Who has a theory of that? What's your theory of the Middle Ages? How could things get dark for so-- so dark for so long? AUDIENCE: Well, I do have a theory. [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: And are we about to have one? AUDIENCE: I think it has as much to do with the channels in which one can communicate ideas to other people-- whether they exist or not, or whether we have [INAUDIBLE] The Middle Ages were characterized by scientific discoveries being kept as family secrets. MARVIN MINSKY: Cardano knew how to take cube roots, and he wouldn't tell anyone. AUDIENCE: Well, the classic stupid example is baby tongs, which for 300 years made a single Italian family very rich. Using tongs to extract the baby in childbirth increased the success rate in difficult births by about 10%, they say. MARVIN MINSKY: Wow. AUDIENCE: And that was enough to build a family fortune, until some servant finally spilled the beans, and that was the the end of that. MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah, wow. So who has a theory of the Middle Ages? Is there a standard theory? Yes? AUDIENCE: Well, the concept of the Middle Ages as the Dark Ages is something that emerged mostly in the Renaissance, when people in the 14-- in the 15th, 16th century tried to present themselves as going back to the Classical Age of scholarship of ancient Greece and Rome and as being better than their predecessors for the last few years. And this mostly happened because of the discovery of manuscripts that were translated from the ancient Greek, and in certain cases Latin, by Muslims, who at the time were receding from Europe. So the entire concept of the Middle Ages might be a fabrication of the Renaissance. There were some significant discoveries at the time. MARVIN MINSKY: That's a nice idea. In other words, when was St. Patrick? AUDIENCE: St. Patrick? MARVIN MINSKY: I'm told that he popularized a lot of technical manuscripts-- brought them back into Europe. He has two achievements. One was bringing scientific culture back, and the other was getting snakes out of England or something-- Ireland. I don't know which he was sainted for. Don't you have to do three miracles, or is it-- what's? Yeah? AUDIENCE: Yeah, my theory is the Middle Ages ended around 2100. So I guess I'll say after the Middle Ages end, they'll say, you know, those guys back in the 21st century, they had no idea how thinking worked. They couldn't even think of a few ways to think. They had poverty. They had wars. Those guys were barely out of their loin cloths. MARVIN MINSKY: Right. I just read a history of AI. I forget who wrote it. But it had this section saying-- it mentioned the Newell-Simon-- there was a thing called General Problem Solver, which I mention a couple of times in the book. And it's the idea that the way to solve a problem is to find-- it's a symbolic servo-- find the difference between what you have and what you want. And look in your memory for something that can reduce that difference. Keep doing that. And, of course, it's important to pay attention to the more important differences first and so forth. And I'll send you this article. This article is saying that they made a terrible mistake, and this was a trivial theory. And that's why nobody uses it anymore. And it was interesting how many AI people fell for that idea in the 1960s. My complaint has been that if you look in a modern textbook on-- you must have some in your first volume, Pat. Didn't you have some GPS things? AUDIENCE: Oh, yeah [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: If you want to keep up with AI, you should read Patrick's textbook, even though people are starting to use this new one, which doesn't have any AI in it. Who is it by? AUDIENCE: Norvig? AUDIENCE: Russell and Norvig. MARVIN MINSKY: Russell and Norvig. It's probably pretty good technically. But I leafed through it, and it didn't have any-- never mind. It's probably better than I think because I'm jealous. Yes? AUDIENCE: Kind of a different topic. In The Society of Mind when you're talking about the amnesia of infancy, when you forget what you've learned, and things that were once difficult become common sense. And you can't even remember how it was you learned that. So I've just been wondering about the reverse of that process, when you try and say teach something to somebody that [INAUDIBLE] bringing back up the different levels. I'm not exactly sure if it was a question. [INAUDIBLE] on it and wondering about is that itself another way of relearning the things that we learned? MARVIN MINSKY: That's sure an interesting question. When I first learned about programming, I had the idea that maybe babies think in machine language. And then after a while, they start to think in Fortran. And then finally, when they're a little older, they think in ALGOL or something. But when they switch from machine language to Fortran, then they can't remember their earlier thoughts. And there's almost no evidence of people finding genuine recollections from two-year-olds at later ages. Now, almost everybody thinks they remember something, but there's the problem that you might have rehearsed it and translated it into the Fortran and the ALGOL and the Lisp and the Logo-- whatever it is. One of my greatest influences was a great mathematician named Andrew Gleason at Harvard, who I met practically the first day I got to Harvard. And he would always talk about things I didn't understand. And I would go home and look them up and try to. Anyway, one day, we were talking about number forms. And number forms are a psychological phenomenon which about 30% of people have. And it was first described by Francis Galton. And the phenomenon is if I ask you close your eyes and tell me where is the number three, how many of you have a place for the number three? Well, that's a few. And so typically, if you imagine the visual field, that's a windshield, I guess. So there are these numbers in there. They're nowhere in particular, except that it's usually like that for an older child. So here are these numbers. And what's more, in some people, they're colored. So I was talking to Andrew. I had read this Galton paper, which was 1890 or 1885 or something. And so I was asking people if they had number forms. And he said, oh, yes, he has one. And he sketched it for me. And he said, and they're colored, too. Oh, and his went way up. And the prime numbers were bright. What am I doing? Maybe the composite numbers were. Something was bright, and they were colored. So I wrote this down. And over the next couple of years, I would look in antiques stores for old children's blocks. And I found a set of blocks that matched that. And Andrew Gleason said that he knows when he acquired this thing, and it was about four years old. And he had a window in his house. And there was a hill. And he could just see over the sill, and he imagined these numbers on the side of that hill-- blah, blah, blah. Anyway, people who don't have a number form don't know what I'm talking about. And I don't know if the 30% is still true. But it's an interesting phenomenon. And in most cases of early childhood-- well, you can't find out because show children do remember details of a house they lived in. But you don't know if they've copied it. So what was the original question? How much can you remember from infancy? L. Ron Hubbard thought you could go back to before you were born, and you could remember people talking about you when you were still in the womb. So anyway, John Campbell said, you should look into this. And a few of us made an expedition. We went down to Elizabeth, New Jersey to visit the just starting up Dianetics Center. And I met this L. Ron Hubbard, who had green eyes and was quite hypnotic-looking. And the end of the story is he had been writing about how if you took this treatment of Dianetics, then you could memorize an entire newspaper in five minutes and do all sorts of miraculous things like that, once your mind has been cleared of aberrations and obstacles. And it became a big industry and turned into, later, Scientology. I'm sure you've all heard about that. So we asked Hubbard to look at a newspaper and tell us what was in it. And he explained he was so busy training the other people to be cleared that he hadn't had time to go through the procedure himself. And I never saw him again. Yes? AUDIENCE: What are your thoughts on memes and the fact that we're just replicas, or our thoughts are actually all replicas of something that-- MARVIN MINSKY: Of memes? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: You mean Dawkins? AUDIENCE: Kind of, yeah. MARVIN MINSKY: I didn't quite get the whole question. The idea that-- AUDIENCE: Oh, so a meme, so for example, the way we talk and the way we all talk probably very much mimics the way our parents talked or people around us and possibly the way we think as well. So how does that relate to how our mind develops? Or are we actually creative original characters, or are we not? [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Well, of course, it's both, because you learn things from your culture. And then you might just mainly repeat things. Or you might get the knack of making new ideas. I'm trying to remember what Dawkins's main ideas are. He invented the word meme to say that the ideas that people have might be considered to be somewhat similar to the genes in our heredity and that societies are systems in which these memes, which are conceptual units of meaning or knowledge, propagate around and self-reproduce and mutate and spread. And I don't know what to say about it, except that it's obviously true that every now and then, someone gets a new idea and tells people. And for one reason or another, they either forget it or tell someone else. And after a while, it spreads. And some of them fill up the whole culture, and some just die out. And whatever else Dawkins says, he's a very smart guy. But almost everything then in my mind is that he's explaining that religions are mostly made of these memes, and they're very bad and cost the world a great deal in progress and productivity. In other words, he's a militant atheist. And there are about five bestsellers in that business today. But I don't know what else to say about memes. It's an obviously generally correct idea. But the great thing about genes is we know the four nucleic acids they're made of and how they're roped together and all that. And I don't think Dawkins's theory develops anywhere nearly as elegantly as modern genetics. So it would be nice if it could, but. A really good theory of good ideas would be nice to have. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: What would it look like? Someday, we'll have an AI that just punches them out. AUDIENCE: I think it would [INAUDIBLE] a really good language. MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, right. Robert Heinlein has some stories in which the super intelligent people have a language that's so dense that in five syllables, they can explain something that would take you a half hour. I forget what it's-- Loglan. Anyway, if you need a good idea, read Robert Heinlein. Sure? AUDIENCE: In [INAUDIBLE] you mentioned this, where you talked about how geniuses, they might have just come up with better ways to think better. Like, better ways to think-- better ways to learn about how to [INAUDIBLE] learning. But why do you think they've never mentioned it? Why haven't they propagated their method of better learning around how [INAUDIBLE] learn? MARVIN MINSKY: That's a great question. And-- AUDIENCE: Do you think [INAUDIBLE] we don't have like a concept of an idea that improves better learning? MARVIN MINSKY: There's a couple of phenomena. Like, how come there were so many geniuses in Athens? And then some of the best mathematicians came from some high school in some little country next to the Baltic. Bulgaria? Romania. Yes, there was some high school in Romania that not only produced von Neumann, but about five or six other world-class mathematicians. I don't remember the details. So that's a nice question. If there are these great memes, how come there aren't more big pockets of them? But there are a lot of cultures which were very inventive in other than intellectual fields. How come Paris got all those artists? And how many of you saw the Woody Allen movie? What's it called? Paris at Midnight? It's so funny. AUDIENCE: Like, would we be able to think of one-- say, one idea that would improve? Your improvements are popular. MARVIN MINSKY: Right. That's a good question for each of us. What's your very best idea? And stop fussing with the other ones, and get that one out. There must be some people who are very quiet and only speak once in a long time. We should watch them carefully. Yes? AUDIENCE: I guess along those lines, do you ever feel restricted by language because [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Wait. It's the sound. I can't hear with it. Is that strong enough to lift you? AUDIENCE: Let's give it a try. MARVIN MINSKY: I'm sure someone's done it. Sorry about that. AUDIENCE: Oh, do you ever feel restricted by language, and that you must represent your theory of mind or any theory or idea with language? MARVIN MINSKY: No, I don't. But I once was jealous when Papert explained that he got some idea, and then he explained that he gets ideas like that when he thinks in French. And-- AUDIENCE: Well, he'd draw pictures, too. MARVIN MINSKY: What? AUDIENCE: He would draw pictures, too. MARVIN MINSKY: Oh, yeah. AUDIENCE: So it's not just language. MARVIN MINSKY: That's a language-- graphics. So we ought to have devices within the next few years that draw pictures when you think. It's so funny. You know, we had cyborgs. What were they called? I mentioned them last time. We had Steve Mann, and who's the other one? AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] MARVIN MINSKY: Yeah. So there's these two guys around the Media Lab wearing various things on their head. And they're always typing. And you ask them a question, and they've searched Google. And when was that? 1990? AUDIENCE: 1995 I think was when Steve Mann was here. MARVIN MINSKY: But it's all gone. And nobody walks around with direct connection to the web. AUDIENCE: Well, Steve Mann still does. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes. Anyway, I certainly expected it to turn up-- something worn. So anyway, you should be able to buy one, one of these days. And what's her name? Who was that nice woman who had the EEG thing? Do you remember at TED? Forgot her name. Anyway, she had this sort of helmet, which had about 20 electrodes. And she induced me to put it on. I was on a stage with about 1,000 people there, which was rather funny. And there's a little spot on a CRT. And I get to think about it moving one way or the other and rewarding it when it did the right thing for only about half a minute. And then I could steer it around. So here was a nice primitive gadget where you could almost draw just by thinking this spot. And then she started a company and hasn't sent me one because maybe it was just beginner's luck or something. AUDIENCE: It was Tan Le. AUDIENCE: Tan Le? MARVIN MINSKY: What? AUDIENCE: Her name-- Tan Le? MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, Le. Yeah. Did you find the company? But you would think there would be lots of people wearing stuff, and-- AUDIENCE: Well, see, she's doing it right now. AUDIENCE: EMOTIV. AUDIENCE: EMOTIV. MARVIN MINSKY: With your keyboard, right. Why don't we take a five-minute break? I don't know. Well, I hate to interrupt because I see 10 different productive discussions. AUDIENCE: I know. MARVIN MINSKY: Yes, I saw quite a few apparently productive discussions. Maybe that's what the class needs. But anybody come to a conclusion? Yeah? AUDIENCE: So I was just resting over here. MARVIN MINSKY: See if you can knock the wall down. Yeah? AUDIENCE: It's not a conclusion, but it's a question. It's a thought experiment. So if you had a black box that could replace part of your brain, and let's say you could replace like 5% at a time. At what point do you assume that there's this self entity? At what point would you lose yourself? MARVIN MINSKY: If you change? The question is how much do you have in common with the you of yesterday as compared to when you graduated grade school? So this question of-- the idea of identity is very, very fuzzy. Yeah? AUDIENCE: So that question sort of reminded me of these peculiar cases of transplantations suddenly having preferences like the person they got the transplant from-- like, heart transplants and stuff. Somehow, they start to like same foods or use the same words, or there's odd things like that. AUDIENCE: Marrying the same spouses. MARVIN MINSKY: I read a science fiction novel by Robert Sawyer. Any of you know of him? A Canadian writer. And it has to do with somebody who has a fatal disease, so he's going to die soon. But the technology is around where you can make a duplicate of him. And so he has a duplicate made. And he is sent to the moon for some reasons I can't remember, which is a kind of a nursing home for, I think, people who are enfeebled and do much better with 1/7 gravity. So there was some reason why. Anyway, the original copy is sent to the moon, and the substitute takes over. But then our hero is miraculously cured by eating the right stem cells or whatever. I don't remember. So he wants to come back. And the question is, who gets the car? So I can't remember the title of the novel, except that it has "alien" in it. Alienable Rights is not-- something like that. But I wrote an article called "Alienable Rights." So are you the same as you were five minutes ago or five years ago or whatever? And as far as I'm concerned, the answer is who cares? It's a sort of silly question because no two things are exactly the same ever anyway. But again, a lot of these questions which look philosophical are legal. So the joke of that novel is that who owns the car is what matters to decide who is the real original and who is the copy. Frederik Pohl wrote a similar story much longer ago, where people are copied. And the copy is sent on a one-way trip to some planet to fix a broken reactor, and they always die. And you get a million dollars for providing this copy. But one of the copies survived, so it's the same plot. I can't remember the-- if you're looking for a good idea, if you go to 1950s science fiction, look for A.E. van Vogt or Frederik Pohl or all those wonderful writers. That was before it was necessary to describe really good characters. And science fiction got better and better for the literary critics and generally worse and worse for the science fiction fans. Do we really have any more questions? Yes? AUDIENCE: So where do you think we [INAUDIBLE] for sharing information between many people short amount of time is by using the internet and everything like this. Do you think that this will change-- this will bring up many more ideas? Do you think that this will hinder it? Because before this time, people had problems with sharing information. And also now, it's much easier to get a large group of people working on one thing. Do you think that this will change the way that we think? And do you think that this will make us [? data ?] [? resource? ?] MARVIN MINSKY: It's a tough one. Bad things can happen, and good things can happen. But that's funny because that reminds me, again, of science fiction because in science fiction, many, many years ago, some writers got the idea that there would be something like an internet. And some people realized there would be flash crowds. And now, there are flash crowds. And I remember even as a kid talking to people who said, why not wire up the voting machines so that they're always there? And so if the somebody in the government wants to know should we do this or that-- should we bomb China or not-- you could get 100 million people to run up to the keyboard and say yes or no. And presumably, when the-- what do they call those? That great crowd of Jeffersons and Franklins and? AUDIENCE: The Founding Fathers. MARVIN MINSKY: The Founding Fathers did a lot of things to prevent that. And the one that they focused on which was one of the most effective was called the Electoral College. And the United States is different from other places because we don't elect congressmen or presidents. We elect smart people from the community who then get together and decide who should be president. And of course, now, if anybody-- now they belong to parties. And if any of them voted for the other party's candidate, there would be hell to pay. But it was a great idea because the Founding Fathers realized that if you had instant feedback, which is what Hitler got, then you could say something really exciting, and everybody would press the "yes" button, and then you kill all the Jews. And then the next speech, you kill all the black people and all the yellow people and all the people whose last name doesn't begin with M. And so what you don't want is instant feedback. Now, the new social networks are getting us close to that. And the question is, is it time to have-- is it time to stop that? Is it getting dangerous? I don't know. But there must be a lot of people who are recognizing that this thing is creeping up on us. And you might be able to get 50 million people to do something reckless in a few minutes if you don't put some limits. I don't think we could get the Electoral College back because you'd have to get a majority to. What does it take to fix the Constitution? 2/3? AUDIENCE: 2/3. MARVIN MINSKY: We'll never see 2/3 again. It's the end of America. Well, we have three minutes. Yes? AUDIENCE: If you were to design a direction for the field of psychology-- and obviously, more than just a set of debugging tools, what do you say they should be doing? MARVIN MINSKY: They should read Patrick Winston's thesis. The psychologists now have disappeared into the tar pit of statistics. And they don't have the idea that knowledge needs complicated representations. And I don't care whether you assign probabilities to them or put them in the order of what you thought of them or do what Doug Lenat did in his thesis of swapping things when one worked better than another. But I forget the question. But I think we've got to get better ideas about representation of knowledge. And I don't know where they're going to come from now that the whole AI community is drifting into these ways of avoiding representations. I haven't read the Norvig-Russell book. Can anybody summarize what it says about knowledge representation? Who's read it? AUDIENCE: Well, there's a chapter on logic and first-order logic. MARVIN MINSKY: That's so funny. AUDIENCE: [INAUDIBLE] logical. MARVIN MINSKY: First-order logic is what Newell and Simon thought of in 1956, before they thought of the so-called GPS thing. Logic can't make analogies. It's a very bad thing to get stuck with, 0 or 1. Maybe one of our papers should be on what should AI do next year? 9 o'clock. Thanks for coming.
Info
Channel: MIT OpenCourseWare
Views: 101,056
Rating: 4.7851958 out of 5
Keywords: love, the self, single-self, infatuation
Id: 6AS48fTXBBs
Channel Id: undefined
Length: 105min 55sec (6355 seconds)
Published: Tue Mar 04 2014
Related Videos
Note
Please note that this website is currently a work in progress! Lots of interesting data and statistics to come.