Professor Paul Bloom:
Okay. The last class we talked about
the brain. Now we're going to talk a
little bit about some foundations.
So today and Monday we're going to talk about two very big ideas
and these ideas are associated with Sigmund Freud and B.
F. Skinner and are psychoanalysis
and behaviorism. And I want to talk about
psychoanalysis today and behaviorism next week.
Now, one of these things--One of the things that makes these
theories so interesting is their scope.
Most of the work we're going to talk about in this class--Most
of the ideas are narrow. So, we're going to talk about
somebody's idea about racial prejudice but that's not a
theory of language acquisition. We'll talk about theories of
schizophrenia but they're not explanations of sexual
attractiveness. Most theories are specialized
theories but these two views are grand theories.
They're theories of everything, encompassing just about
everything that matters, day-to-day life,
child development, mental illness,
religion, war, love.
Freud and Skinner had explanations of all of these.
Now, this is not a history course.
I have zero interest in describing historical figures in
psychology just for the sake of telling you about the history of
the field. What I want to tell you about
though is--I want to talk about these ideas because so much
rests on them and, even more importantly,
a lot of these ideas have critical influence on how we
think about the present. And that's there.
Now, for better or worse, we live in a world profoundly
affected by Sigmund Freud. If I had to ask you to choose
a--no, name a famous psychologist,
the answer of most of you would be Freud.
He's the most famous psychologist ever and he's had a
profound influence on the twentieth and twenty-first
century. Some biographical information:
He was born in the 1850s. He spent most of his life in
Vienna, Austria, but he died in London and he
escaped to London soon after retreating there at the
beginning of World War II as the Nazis began to occupy where he
lived. He's one of the most famous
scholars ever but he's not known for any single discovery.
Instead, he's known for the development of an encompassing
theory of mind, one that he developed over the
span of many decades. He was in his time extremely
well known, a celebrity recognized on the street,
and throughout his life. He was a man of extraordinary
energy and productivity, in part because he was a very
serious cocaine addict, but also just in general.
He was just a high-energy sort of person.
He was up for the Nobel Prize in medicine and in literature;
didn't get either one of them; didn't get the prize in
medicine because Albert Einstein--Everybody loves Albert
Einstein. Well, Albert Einstein really
wrote a letter because they asked for opinions of other
Nobel Prizes. He wrote a letter saying,
"Don't give the prize to Freud. He doesn't deserve a Nobel
Prize. He's just a psychologist."
Well, yeah. Okay.
While he's almost universally acclaimed as a profoundly
important intellectual figure, he's also the object of
considerable dislike. This is in part because of his
character. He was not a very nice man in
many ways. He was deeply ambitious to the
cause of promoting psychoanalysis,
to the cause of presenting his view and defending it,
and he was often dishonest, extremely brutal to his
friends, and terrible to his enemies.
He was an interesting character. My favorite Freud story was as
he was leaving Europe during the rise of the Nazis,
as he was ready to go to England from,
I think, either Germany or Austria, he had to sign a letter
from the Gestapo. Gestapo agents intercepted him
and demanded he sign a letter saying that at no point had he
been threatened or harassed by the Gestapo.
So he signs the letter and then he writes underneath it,
"The Gestapo has not harmed me in any way.
In fact, I highly recommend the Gestapo to everybody."
It's--He had a certain aggression to him.
He was also--He's also disliked, often hated,
because of his views. He was seen as a sexual
renegade out to destroy the conception of people as good and
rational and pure beings. And when the Nazis rose to
power in the 1930s he was identified as a Jew who was
devoted to destroying the most sacred notions of Christianity
and to many, to some extent,
many people see him this way. And to some extent,
this accusation has some truth to it.
Freud made claims about people that many of us,
maybe most of us, would rather not know.
Well, okay. What did he say?
Well, if you ask somebody who doesn't like Freud what he said,
they'll describe some of the stupider things he said and,
in fact, Freud said a lot of things, some of which were not
very rational. For instance,
he's well known for his account of phallic symbols,
arguing certain architectural monuments are subconsciously
developed as penile representations.
And related to this, he developed the notorious
theory of penis envy. And penis envy is an account of
a developmental state that every one of you who is female has
gone through, according to Freud.
And the idea is that you discovered at some point in your
development that you lacked a penis.
This is not--This is a catastrophe.
And so, each of you inferred at that point that you had been
castrated. You had once had a penis but
somebody had taken it from you. You then turn to your father
and love your father because your father has a penis,
so he's a sort of penis substitute.
You reject your mother, who's equally unworthy due to
her penis lack, and that shapes your
psychosexual development. Now, if that's the sort of
thing you know about Freud, you are not going to have a
very high opinion of him or of his work,
but at the core of Freud's declamation, the more
interesting ideas, is a set of claims of a man's
intellectual importance. And the two main ones are this.
The two main ones involve the existence of an unconscious,
unconscious motivation, and the notion of unconscious
dynamics or unconscious conflict which lead to mental illnesses,
dreams, slips of the tongue and so on.
The first idea β the idea of unconscious motivation β
involves rejecting the claim that you know what you're doing.
So, suppose you fall in love with somebody and you decide you
want to marry them and then somebody was asked to ask you
why and you'd say something like,
"Well, I'm ready to get married this stage of my life;
I really love the person; the person is smart and
attractive; I want to have kids" whatever.
And maybe this is true. But a Freudian might say that
even if this is your honest answer β you're not lying to
anybody else βstill, there are desires and
motivations that govern your behavior that you may not be
aware of. So, in fact,
you might want to marry John because he reminds you of your
father or because you want to get back at somebody for
betraying you. If somebody was to tell you
this, you'd say, "That's total nonsense," but
that wouldn't deter a Freudian. The Freudian would say that
these processes are unconscious so of course you just don't know
what's happening. So, the radical idea here is
you might not know what--why you do what you do and this is
something we accept for things like visual perception.
We accept that you look around the world and you get sensations
and you figure out there is a car, there is a tree,
there is a person. And you're just unconscious of
how this happens but it's unpleasant and kind of
frightening that this could happen,
that this could apply to things like why you're now studying at
Yale, why you feel the way you do towards your friends,
towards your family. Now, the marriage case is
extreme but Freud gives a lot of simpler examples where this sort
of unconscious motivation might play a role.
So, have you ever liked somebody or disliked them and
not known why? Have you ever found yourself in
a situation where you're doing something or you're arguing for
something or making a decision for reasons that you can't fully
articulate? Have you ever forgotten
somebody's name at exactly the wrong time?
Have you ever called out the wrong name in the throes of
passion? This is all the Freudian
unconscious. The idea is that we do these
things--these things are explained in terms of cognitive
systems that we're not aware of. Now, all of this would be fine
if your unconscious was a reasonable, rational computer,
if your unconscious was really smart and looking out for your
best interest. But, according to Freud,
that's not the way it works. According to Freud,
there are three distinct processes going on in your head
and these are in violent internal conflict.
And the way you act and the way you think are products,
not of a singular rational being, but of a set of
conflicting creatures. And these three parts are the
id, the ego, and the superego and they emerge developmentally.
The id, according to Freud, is present at birth.
It's the animal part of the self.
It wants to eat, drink, pee, poop,
get warm, and have sexual satisfaction.
It is outrageously stupid. It works on what Freud called,
"The Pleasure Principle." It wants pleasure and it wants
it now. And that's, according to Freud,
how a human begins β pure id. Freud had this wonderful
phrase, "polymorphous perversity," this pure desire
for pleasure. Now, unfortunately,
life doesn't work like that. What you want isn't always what
you get and this leads to a set of reactions to cope with the
fact that pleasure isn't always there when you want it either by
planning how to satisfy your desires or planning how to
suppress them. And this system is known as the
ego, or the self. And it works on the "Reality
Principle." And it works on the principle
of trying to figure out how to make your way through the world,
how to satisfy your pleasures or, in some cases,
how to give up on them. And the ego β the emergence
of the ego for Freud--symbolizes the origin of consciousness.
Finally, if this was all there it might be a simpler world,
but Freud had a third component, that of the superego.
And the superego is the internalized rules of parents in
society. So, what happens in the course
of development is, you're just trying to make your
way through the world and satisfy your desires,
but sometimes you're punished for them.
Some desires are inappropriate, some actions are wrong,
and you're punished for it. The idea is that you come out;
you get in your head a superego, a conscience.
In these movies, there'd be a little angel above
your head that tells you when things are wrong.
And basically your self, the ego, is in between the id
and the superego. One thing to realize,
I told you the id is outrageously stupid.
It just says, "Oh, hungry,
food, sex, oh, let's get warm,
oh." The superego is also stupid.
The superego, point to point,
is not some brilliant moral philosopher telling you about
right and wrong. The superego would say,
"You should be ashamed of yourself.
That's disgusting. Stop doing that.
Oh." And in between these two
screaming creatures, one of you;
one of them telling you to seek out your desires,
the other one telling you, "you should be ashamed of
yourself," is you, is the ego.
Now, according to Freud, most of this is unconscious.
So, we see bubbling up to the top, we feel,
we experience ourselves. And the driving of the id,
the forces of the id and the forces of the superego,
are unconscious in that we cannot access them.
We don't know what--It's like the workings of our kidneys or
our stomachs. You can't introspect and find
them. Rather, they do their work
without conscious knowledge. Now, Freud developed this.
This is the Freudian theory in broad outline.
He extended it and developed it into a theory of psychosexual
development. And so, Freud's theory is,
as I said before, a theory of everyday life,
of decisions, of errors, of falling in love,
but it's also a theory of child development.
So, Freud believed there were five stages of personality
development, and each is associated with a particular
erogenous zone. And Freud believed,
as well, that if you have a problem at a certain stage,
if something goes wrong, you'll be stuck there.
So, according to Freud, there are people in this room
who are what they are because they got stuck in the oral stage
or the anal stage. And that's not good.
So, the oral stage is when you start off.
The mouth is associated with pleasure.
Everything is sucking and chewing and so on.
And the problem for Freud is premature weaning of a child.
Depriving him of the breast, could lead to serious problems
in his personality development. It could make him,
as the phrase goes, into an oral person.
And his orality could be described literally.
Freud uses it as an explanation for why somebody might eat too
much or chew gum or smoke. They're trying to achieve
satisfaction through their mouth of a sort they didn't get in
this very early stage of development.
But it can also be more abstract.
If your roommate is dependent and needy, you could then go to
your roommate and say, "You are an oral person.
The first year of your life did not go well."
A phrase even more popular is the anal stage and that happens
after the oral stage. And problems can emerge if
toilet training is not handled correctly.
If you have problems during those years of life,
you could become an anal personality,
according to Freud, and your roommate could say,
"Your problem is you're too anal."
And, according to Freud, literally, it meant you are
unwilling to part with your own feces. It's written down here.
I know it's true. And the way it manifests
itself, as you know from just how people talk,
is you're compulsive, you're clean,
you're stingy. This is the anal personality.
Then it gets a little bit more complicated.
The next stage is the phallic stage.
Actually, this is not much more complicated.
The focus of pleasure shifts to the genitals and fixation can
lead to excessive masculinity in females or in males or if you're
female a need for attention or domination.
Now, at this point something really interesting happens
called the "Oedipus Complex." And this is based on the story,
the mythical story of a king who killed his father and
married his mother. And, according to Freud,
this happens to all of us in this way.
Well, all of us. By "all of us," Freud meant
"men." So, here's the idea.
You're three or four years old. You're in the phallic stage.
So, what are you interested in? Well, you're interested in your
penis and then you seek an external object.
Freud's sort of vague about this, but you seek some sort of
satisfaction. But who is out there who'd be
sweet and kind and loving and wonderful?
Well, Mom. So the child infers,
"Mom is nice, I love Mom."
So far so--And so this is not crazy;
a little boy falling in love with his mother.
Problem: Dad's in the way. Now, this is going to get
progressively weirder but I will have to say, as the father of
two sons, both sons went through a phase
where they explicitly said they wanted to marry Mommy.
And me β if something bad happened to me that wouldn't be
the worst thing in the world. So, there's this.
But now it gets a little bit aggressive.
So, the idea is the child determines that he's going to
kill his father. Every three- and four-year-old
boy thinks this. But then because children,
according to Freud, don't have a good sense of the
boundary between their mind and the world,
which is a problem β the problem is they don't β they
think their father can tell that they're plotting to kill him and
they figure their father is now angry at them.
And then they ask themselves, "What's the worst thing Dad
could do to me?" And the answer is castration.
So, they come to the conclusion that their father is going to
castrate them because of their illicit love for their Mom.
And then they say, "Dad wins" and then they don't
think about sex for several years and that's the latency
stage. The latency stage is they've
gone through this huge thing with Mom and Dad,
"fell in love with Mom, wanted to kill my father,
Dad was going to castrate me, fell out of love with Mom,
out of the sex business." And then, sex is repressed
until you get to the genital stage.
And the genital stage is the stage we are all in β the
healthy adult stage. Now that you're adults and
you've gone through all the developmental stages,
where do you stand? You're not out of the woods yet
because unconscious mechanisms are still--Even if you haven't
got fixated on anything, there's still this dynamic
going on all the time with your id, your ego and your superego.
And the idea is your superego--Remember,
your superego is stupid. So, your superego isn't only
telling you not to do bad things, it's telling you not to
think bad things. So, what's happening is your id
is sending up all of this weird, sick stuff, all of these crazy
sexual and violent desires, "Oh, I'll kill him.
I'll have sex with that. I'll have extra helpings on my
dessert." And your superego is saying,
"No, no, no." And this stuff is repressed.
It doesn't even make it to consciousness.
The problem is Freud had a very sort of hydraulic theory of what
goes on and some of this stuff slips out and it shows up in
dreams and it shows up in slips of the tongue.
And in exceptional cases, it shows up in certain clinical
symptoms. So what happens is,
Freud described a lot of normal life in terms of different ways
we use to keep that horrible stuff from the id making its way
to consciousness. And he called these "defense
mechanisms." You're defending yourself
against the horrible parts of yourself and some of these make
a little bit of sense. One way to describe this in a
non-technical, non-Freudian way is,
there are certain things about ourselves we'd rather not know.
There are certain desires we'd rather not know and we have ways
to hide them. So, for instance,
there's sublimation. Sublimation is you might have a
lot of energy, maybe sexual energy or
aggressive energy, but instead of turning it to a
sexual or aggressive target what you do is you focus it in some
other way. So, you can imagine a great
artist like Picasso turning the sexual energy into his artwork.
There is displacement. Displacement is you have
certain shameful thoughts or desires and you refocus them
more appropriately. A boy who's bullied by his
father may hate his father and want to hurt him but since this
would--this is very shameful and difficult.
The boy might instead kick the dog and think he hates the dog
because that's a more acceptable target. There is projection.
Projection is, I have certain impulses I am
uncomfortable with, so rather than own them myself,
I project them to somebody else.
A classic example for Freud is homosexual desires.
The idea is that I feel this tremendous lust towards you,
for instance, and--any of you,
all of you, you three, and I'm ashamed of this lust so
what I say is, "Hey.
Are you guys looking at me in a sexual manner?
Are you lusting after me? How disgusting," because what I
do is I take my own desires and I project it to others.
And Freud suggested, perhaps not implausibly,
that men who believe other men--who are obsessed with the
sexuality of other men, are themselves projecting away
their own sexual desires. There is rationalization,
which is that when you do something or think something bad
you rationalize it and you give it a more socially acceptable
explanation. A parent who enjoys smacking
his child will typically not say, "I enjoy smacking my
child." Rather he'll say,
"It's for the child's own good. I'm being a good parent by
doing this." And finally,
there is regression, which is returning to an
earlier stage of development. And you actually see this in
children. In times of stress and trauma,
they'll become younger, they will act younger.
They might cry. They might suck their thumb,
seek out a blanket or so on. Now, these are all mechanisms
that for Freud are not the slightest bit pathological.
They are part of normal life. Normally, we do these things to
keep an equilibrium among the different systems of the
unconscious, but sometimes it doesn't work.
Sometimes things go awry and what happens is a phrase that's
not currently used in psychology but was popular during Freud's
time: hysteria. Hysteria includes phenomena
like hysterical blindness and hysterical deafness,
which is when you cannot see and cannot hear even though
there's nothing physiologically wrong with you β paralysis,
trembling, panic attacks, gaps of memory including
amnesia and so on. And the idea is that these are
actually symptoms. These are symptoms of
mechanisms going on to keep things unconscious.
It's a common enough idea in movies.
Often in movies what happens is that somebody goes to an
analyst. They have some horrible problem.
They can't remember something or they have some sort of
blackouts and so on. And the analyst tells them
something and at one point they get this insight and they
realize what--why they've blinded themselves,
why they can't remember, and for Freud this is what
happens. Freud originally attempted to
get these memories out through hypnosis but then moved to the
mechanism of free association and,
according to Freud, the idea is patients offer
resistance to this and then the idea of a psychoanalyst is to
get over the resistance and help patients get insight.
The key notion of psychoanalysis is your problems
are--actually reflect deeper phenomena.
You're hiding something from yourself, and once you know
what's going on to deeper phenomena your problems will go
away. I'm going to give you an
example of a therapy session. Now, this is not a Freudian
analysis. We'll discuss later on in the
course what a Freudian analysis is, but this is not a pure
Freudian analysis. A Freudian analysis,
of course, is lying on a couch; does not see their therapist;
their therapist is very nondirective.
But I'm going to present this as an example here because it
illustrates so many of the Freudian themes,
particularly themes about dreams, the importance of
dreams, about repression and about hidden meaning.
So, this is from a television episode and the
character's--Many--Some of you may have seen this.
Many of you will not have. The character is suffering from
panic attacks. Freud's contributions extend
beyond the study of individual psychology and individual
pathology. Freud had a lot to say about
dreams as you could see in this illustration.
He believed that dreams had a manifest content,
meaning; "manifest" meaning what you
experience in your dream. But dreams always had a latent
content as well, meaning the hidden implication
of the dream. He viewed all dreams as wish
fulfillment. Every dream you have is a
certain wish you have even though it might be a forbidden
wish that you wouldn't wish to have, you wouldn't want to have.
And dreams had--and this is an idea that long predated Freud.
Dreams had symbolism. Things in dreams were often not
what they seemed to be but rather symbols for other things.
Freud believed that literature and fairy tales and stories to
children and the like carried certain universal themes,
certain aspects of unconscious struggles, and certain
preoccupations of our unconscious mind.
And Freud had a lot to say about religion.
For instance, he viewed a large part of
our--of the idea of finding a singular,
all-powerful god as seeking out a father figure that some of us
never had during development. What I want to spend the rest
of the class on is the scientific assessment of Freud.
So, what I did so far is I've told you what Freud had to say
in broad outline. I then want to take the time to
consider whether or not we should believe this and how well
it fits with our modern science. But before doing so,
I'll take questions for a few minutes.
Do people have any questions about Freud or Freud's theories? Yes.
Student: [inaudible]
Professor Paul Bloom: So, that's some question.
The question is: The conflicts in psychosexual
development that Freud describes is--always assumes that a child
has a mother and a father, one of each,
in a certain sort of familial structure.
And the question then is, "What if a child was raised by
a single parent, for example?"
What if a child was never breast fed, but fed from the
bottle from the start? And Freudians have had problems
with this. Freud's--Freud was very focused
on the family life of the people he interacted with,
which is rather upper class Europeans, and these sort of
questions would have been difficult for Freud to answer.
I imagine that what a Freudian would have to say is,
you would expect systematic differences.
So, you would expect a child who just grew up with a mother
or just grew up to be a father--with a father to be in
some sense psychologically damaged by that,
failing to go through the normal psychosexual stages.
Yes. Student:
[inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:
The issue--The question is, "Do modern psychoanalysts still
believe that women do not have superegos?"
Freud was--As you're pointing out, Freud was notorious for
pointing, for suggesting that women were morally immature
relative to men. I think Freud would say that
women have superegos, they're just not the sort of
sturdy ones that men have. I think psychoanalysts and
psychoanalytic scholars right now would be mixed.
Some would maintain that there really are deep sex differences.
Others would want to jettison that aspect of Freudian theory. Yes.
Student: Do you define sublimation as
being displacement? Does that make it sort of a
subgroup of displacement? Professor Paul Bloom:
Well, what sublimation is--A lot of these--It's a good
question. The question is sort of,
what is sublimation? How does it relate to the other
defense mechanisms? A lot of defense mechanisms
involve taking a desire and turning it.
Now, what displacement does is it takes it from you to her.
I'm angry at you but maybe that's forbidden for some
reason, so I'll be angry at her. What projection does is takes a
desire from me and then puts it on somebody else heading
outwards. And what sublimation does is it
just gives up the details and keeps the energy.
So, you stay up--Your roommate stays up all night working and
you say to your roommate, for instance,
"That's just because you haven't had sex in a long time
and you want to have sex so you devote all your energy to your
math exam." And then you say,
"That's sublimation. I learned that in Intro Psych."
And your roommate would be very pleased.
One more question. Yes.
Student: What kind of evidence is there
for cross-cultural variation? Professor Paul Bloom:
The question is, which is related to the
issue--extending the issue of the two-parent versus one-parent
family is, "To what extent are these
notions validated cross-culturally?"
And that's such a good question I'm going to defer it.
I'm going to talk about it in a few minutes because that's
actually--That speaks to the issue of the scientific
assessment of Freud so I'm going to try to get to your question
in a little bit. Freudian theory is now,
at this point of time, extremely controversial and
there is a lot of well-known criticisms and attacks on Freud.
This is just actually an excellent book on The Memory
Wars by Frederick Crews, which--and Frederick Crews is
one of the strongest and most passionate critics of Freud.
And the problems with Freud go like this.
There are two ways you could reject a theory.
There are two problems with the scientific theory.
One way you could reject a theory is that it could be
wrong. So, suppose I have a theory
that the reason why some children have autism,
a profound developmental disorder,
is because their mothers don't love them enough.
This was a popular theory for many years.
It's a possible theory. It just turns out to be wrong
but another way--And so one way to attack and address a
scientific theory is to view it as just to see whether or not it
works. But there's a different problem
a theory could have. A theory could be so vague and
all encompassing that it can't even be tested.
And this is one of the main critiques of Freud.
The idea could be summed up by a quotation from the physicist
Wolfgang Pauli. And Pauli was asked his opinion
about another physicist. And Pauli said this:
"That guy's work is crap. He's not right.
He's not even wrong." And the criticism about Freud
is that he's not even wrong. The issue of vagueness is
summarized in a more technical way by the philosopher Karl
Popper who described--who introduced the term of
falsifiability. The idea of falsifiability is
that what distinguishes science from non science is that
scientific predictions make strong claims about the world
and these claims are of a sort that they could be proven wrong.
If they couldn't be proven wrong, they're not interesting
enough to be science. So, for example,
within psychology the sort of claims we'll be entertaining
throughout the course include claims like,
damage to the hippocampus causes failures of certain sorts
of memory, or everywhere in the world men on average want to
have more sexual partners than women,
or exposure to violent television tends to make
children themselves more violent.
Now, are they true or are they false?
Well, we'll talk about that, but the point here is they can
be false. They're interesting enough that
they can be tested and as such they go to--they might be wrong
but they graduate to the level of a scientific theory.
This should be contrasted with nonscientific programs and the
best example of a nonscientific program is astrology.
So, the problem with astrological predictions is not
that they're wrong. It's that they can't be wrong.
They're not even wrong. I did my--I got my horoscope
for today on the web. "A couple of negative aspects
could make you a little finicky for the next few days."
Okay. I'm going to watch for that.
"The presence of both Mars and Venus suggests you want to box
everything into a neat, ordered, structured way but
keeping a piece of jade or carnelian close will help you
keep in touch with your fun side."
And starting this morning I got from my wife a little piece of
jade and I have been sort of in touch with my fun side.
The problem is, a few days aren't going to go
by and say, "God. That was wrong."
It can't be wrong. It's just so vague.
I got a better horoscope from The Onion actually:
"Riding in a golf cart with snow cone in hand,
you'll be tackled by two police officers this week after
matching a composite caricature of a suspected murderer."
Now, that's a good prediction because "wow."
If it turns out to be true, I'm going to say,
"Those guys really know something."
It's falsifiable. Arguably, Freud fails the test
because Freudian theory is often so vague and flexible that it
can't really be tested in any reliable way.
A big problem with this is a lot of Freudian theory is
claimed to be validated in the course of psychoanalysis.
So, when you ask people, "Why do you believe in Freud?"
they won't say, "Oh, because of this
experiment, that experiment, this data set and that data
set." What they'll say is,
"It's--The Freudian theory proves itself in the course of
psychoanalysis β the success of psychoanalysis."
But it's unreliable. The problem is,
say, Freud says to a patient, "You hate your mother."
The patient says, "Wow. That makes sense."
Freud says, "I'm right." The patient--Freud says,
"You hate your mother," and the patient says,
"No, I don't. That's titillating.
That's disgusting." Freud says, "Your anger shows
this idea is painful to you. You have repressed it from
consciousness. I am right."
And the problem is the same sort of dynamic plays itself out
even in the scientific debate back and forth.
So Freud--Freudian psychologists--I'm putting Freud
here but what I mean is well-known defenders of Freud
will make some claims like: adult personality traits are
shaped by the course of psychosexual development;
all dreams are disguised wish fulfillment;
psychoanalysis is the best treatment for mental disorders.
Scientists will respond, "I disagree.
There's little or no evidence supporting those claims."
And the Freudian response is, "Your rejection of my ideas
shows that they are distressing to you.
This is because I am right." And this is often followed up,
seriously enough. "You have deep psychological
problems." And now, I don't want to
caricature Freudians. A lot of Freudians have tried
and made a research program of extending their ideas
scientifically, bringing them to robust
scientific tests. But the problem is,
when you make specific falsifiable predictions they
don't always do that well. So, for instance,
there's no evidence that oral and anal characteristics,
the personality characteristics I talked about β about being
needy versus being stingy β relate in any interesting way to
weaning or toilet training. And there's been some efforts
cross-culturally, to go back to the question this
young man asked before β looking at cross-cultural
differences in toilet training and weaning,
which are really big differences, to see if they
correspond in any interesting way to personality differences.
And there's been no good evidence supporting that.
Similarly, Freud had some strong claims about sexuality,
for why some people are straight and others are gay.
These have met with very little empirical support.
And the claim that psychoanalysis proves itself by
being--by its tremendous success in curing mental illness is also
almost certainly not true. For most--Maybe not all,
but for most psychological disorders, there are quicker and
more reliable treatments than psychoanalysis.
And there's considerable controversy as to whether the
Tony Soprano method of insight, where you get this insight and
there's discovery, "Oh, now I know," makes any
real difference in alleviating symptoms such as anxiety
disorders or depression. This is why there's sort
of--often sort of a sticker shock when people go to a
university psychology department where they say,
"Look. Hey.
Where is--So I'm in Psych. How could I take classes on
Freud? Who's your expert on Freud?"
And the truth is Freudian psychoanalysis is almost never
studied inside psychology departments.
Not the cognitive or developmental side,
not the clinical side. There are some exceptions but,
for the most part, even the people who do study
Freud within psychology departments do so critically.
Very few of them would see themselves as a psychoanalytic
practitioner or as a Freudian psychologist.
Freud lives on both in a clinical setting and in the
university but Freud at Yale, for instance,
is much more likely to be found in the history department or the
literature department than in the psychology department.
And this is typical enough but, despite all of the,
sort of, sour things I just said about Freud,
the big idea, the importance of the dynamic
unconscious, remains intact. We will go over and over and
over again different case studies where some really
interesting aspects of mental life prove to be unconscious.
Now, there's one question. I'm actually going to skip over
this for reasons of time and just go to some examples of the
unconscious in modern psychology.
So, here's a simple example of the unconscious in modern
psychology: Language understanding.
So, when you hear a sentence like, "John thinks that Bill
likes him," in a fraction of a second you realize that this
means that John thinks that Bill likes John.
If you heard the sentence--Oops--"John thinks
that Bill likes himself," in a fraction of a second you would
think that it means "John thinks that Bill likes Bill."
And as we will get to when we get to the lecture on language,
this is not conscious. You don't know how you do this.
You don't even know that you are doing this but you do it
quickly and instinctively. So much of our day-to-day life
can be done unconsciously. There are different activities
you can do β driving, chewing gum,
shoelace tying β where if you're good enough at them,
if you're expert enough at them, you don't know you're
doing them. I was at a party a few years
ago for a friend of mine and we ran out of food so he said,
"I'll just go pick up some food."
An hour later he was gone--still gone and it was
around the corner. And we called him up on his
cell phone and he said, "Oh.
I got on the highway and I drove to work."
Yeah. He works an hour away but he
got on the highway "drive drive drive."
And these--some version of these things happen all of the
time. Maybe more surprising,
Freud's insight that our likes and dislikes are due to factors
that we're not necessarily conscious of has a lot of
empirical support--a lot of empirical support from research
into social psychology, for example.
So, here's one finding from social psychology.
If somebody goes through a terrible initiation to get into
a club, they'll like the club more.
You might think they'd like it less because people do terrible
things to them. But actually,
hazing is illegal but a remarkably successful tool.
The more you pay for something the more you like it and the
more pain you go through to get something the more you like it.
From the standpoint of politics for instance,
if you want loyal people in a political campaign,
do not pay them. If you pay them,
they'll like you less. If they volunteer,
they'll like you more. And we'll talk about why.
There's different theories about why, but my point right
now is simply that people don't necessarily know this but still
they're subject to this. Another example is some weird
studies done in a discipline of social psychology known as
terror management which involves subliminal death primes.
The idea of subliminal death primes is this.
You sign up for your human subjects requirement and then
you--they put you in front of a computer screen and then they
tell you, "Oh, just sit in front of the
computer screen and then we'll ask you some questions."
And then the questions come out and they're questions like,
"How much do you love your country?"
"What do you think of Asians?" "What do you think of Jews?"
"What do you think of blacks?" "What do you think of
vegetarians?" "What do you think of people's
political views different from yours?"
Here's the gimmick. What you don't know is on that
computer screen words are being flashed like that but they're
being flashed so fast it looks like that--You don't see
anything--words like "corpse," "dead," "dying."
The flashing of these subliminal words,
"subliminal" meaning β a fancy term meaning below the
level of consciousness, you don't know you're seeing
them β has dramatic effects on how you answer those questions.
People exposed to death primes become more nationalistic,
more patriotic, less forgiving of other people,
less liking of other races and people from other countries.
Again the claim--the explanation for why this is so
is something which we'll get to in another class.
The point now is simply to illustrate that these sort of
things can have--that things you aren't aware of can have an
effect on how you think. The final example I'll give of
this is a short demonstration. To do this, I'm going to cut
the class in half at this point so you'll be on this side of the
class, the right side,
my right, and this will be on the left side,
and I simply want everybody to think about somebody you love.
So, think about somebody you love, your girlfriend,
your boyfriend, your mom, your dad.
Think about somebody you love. Just think.
Okay. Now, on this screen is going to
be instructions but I want to give the instructions to this
half of the class . I'm going to ask everybody in
this half of the class please either turn your head or shut
your eyes. Okay?
Teaching fellows too. Okay.
And everybody on this half obey .
Okay. Has everybody read that?
Okay. Now, turn your head, this group.
Now this group: Look at this and take a moment.
You don't have to do it on paper but take a moment to do it
in your head. You--Each group had
instructions. Some people might have seen
both instructions. Follow the instructions you got
for you. Now, this was research done by
Norbert Schwarz and here's the question I want you to ask
yourself, "How much do you like this person?"
And here's the effect: Half of you were asked to list
three features of the person. Half of you were asked to list
ten. The finding,
which is not a subtle finding, is that liking goes up in the
three group and liking goes down in the ten group.
And here is why. I have to think about three
positive features of somebody so I think about my girlfriend.
I have a girlfriend. I think about my girlfriend,
"but oh, she's smart, she's beautiful and she's kind.
Good. How much do--What do I think of
her? "Pretty, good,
smart, beautiful, kind, smart,
beautiful, oh, yeah."
But the problem--;Now, Schwarz is clever though.
He says, "List--" The other group gets ten positive
features, "smart, beautiful,
kind⦠really nice⦠good cook⦠punctual,
smart⦠No, I mentioned that."
The problem is nobody has ten positive features!
And the effect of being asked to do ten positive features is
people find this hard. And then those people,
when asked, "How much do you like this person?"
say, "Couldn't really make it that ten.
I guess I don't like them very much."
Now, the point of this illustration,
again, is that it shows that you don't know this.
Subjects who were asked to do ten positive features and then
later ranked the person lower and then asked,
"Why did you rank the person lower?"
Don't say, "'Cause you told me to list ten."
Typically, we are oblivious to these factors that change our
points β what we like and what we dislike β and this is,
in fact, a substantial and an important part of the study of
psychology, and particularly, for instance,
the study of racial and sexual prejudice.
Where--One of the big findings from social psychology,
and we'll devote almost an entire lecture to this,
is that people have strong views about other races that
they don't know about and that they don't know how to control
their actions. So, to some extent,
this rounds out Freud because to some extent the particulars
of Freud are--for the most part have been rejected.
But the general idea of Freud's actually been so successful both
in the study of scientific psychology and in our
interpretation of everyday life that,
to some extent, Freud's been a victim of his
own success. We tend to underestimate the
importance of Freudian thought in everyday life because he's
transformed our world view to such an extent that it's
difficult for us to remember if there's any other way to think
about it. So, to some extent,
he's been the victim of his own success.
We have time for some further questions about Freud and about
scientific implications of Freud. I took a class once on how to
teach when I was a graduate student.
And I just remember two things from this class.
One thing is never grade in red pen.
Those--People don't like that. The second thing is never ask
any questions, because presumably it is very
frightening to ask, "Any questions?"
and people find it's intimidating.
I'm supposed to ask, "What are your questions?"
So, what are your questions? Yes, in back.
Sorry. Student:
Did Freud believe in [inaudible]
Professor Paul Bloom: Did Freud believe in
[inaudible] Student:
Medication Professor Paul Bloom: Medication.
Freud had an--It's a good question.
The question was, "Does--did Freud believe in
medication?" Medication, of course,
being a major theme of how we deal with certain disorders now,
particularly depression and anxiety disorders.
On the one hand, Freud made his start as a
neuroscientist. Freud studied the mind and the
brain and was intensely interested in the neural basis
of thought and behavior. But the answer to your question
in the end is, "no."
Although Freud was very sensitive to the brain basis of
behavior, Freud was totally convinced that the method
through which to cure disorders like depression and anxiety
would not be medication but rather through the sort of talk
therapy and insight. Moreover, modern therapists,
including some people who aren't psychoanalytically
defined, will say, "Look.
These drugs are all well and good but what they do is they
mask the symptoms." So, if you have panic attacks,
say, it's true that drugs might make the panic attacks go away,
but the panic attacks are actually not your real problem.
And by making them go away you don't get to the root of your
problem. So, the answer is both Freud
and modern day psychoanalysts would think that medications are
substantially overused in the treatment of mental disorders.
Yes. Student:
Are there any [inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:
The question is, "What about research on
dreams?" "Dreams" is such a fun topic
that I'm going to devote half a class to sleeping and dreams.
So, for instance, I will answer the question
"What is the most common dream?" I will also answer the question
"Who thinks about sex more in dreams, men or women,
and what proportion of--" Oh. There's so many great questions
I will answer. Dreams from a Freudian
standpoint. There's been some evidence that
dreams do, and some often do, have some relationship to what
you're thinking about and worrying about through the day.
But the strong Freudian view about symbolism and wish
fulfillment has not been supported by the study of
dreams. What are your other questions?
Yes, whoever Erik is pointing to.
Professor Paul Bloom: Purple shirt.
Yes. Student:
[inaudible]--Electra complex? Professor Paul Bloom:
The Electra complex? The Electra complex is the
penis envy story. Freud developed--This is a
crude summary, but Freud developed the Oedipal
complex, "Mom, I love Mommy,
Dad." And then it's as if somebody
reminded him, "Sigmund, there are also
women." "Oh, yeah."
And that story I told you with the penises and the penis envy
and the replacement is sort of a very shortened version of the
Electra complex. I think it's fair to say that
the Electra complex was a sort of add-on to the main interest
of Freud's Oedipal complex. One more, please.
Yes. Student:
[inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:
According to Freud, the--there's not a fixation in
the stage, in the same sense as an oral or anal stage,
but yes. The claim that Freud would make
is that the woman's discovery that she lacks the penis plays a
fundamental role later on determining her allegiances in
life and in fact her own sexual preferences and interests.
So, it's not the sort of thing that affects her just for a
short period.
Woah...vid is 56 min long. I'll take your word for it
You tell that guy he should fuck his mother once and you never live it down.
Einstein was absolutely right in doing so.
I wish I could down vote this to hell by myself simply because it doesn't state where in this hour-long video this fact is mentioned
Freud was a very interesting fraud. A large portion of his ideas were proven to be interesting and wrong and his methods were atrocious.
Sigmund Freud was actually nominated three times for the Nobel Prize, both in Literature and in Medicine, which is quite unique in itself. Source: http://www.scribd.com/doc/6482722/Nobel-Prize-and-Psychology
Well Freud did blow and had sex with his patients, so Einstein was probably jealous.